Problem-Solving and Reasoning Chapter 12 Problem-Solving and Reasoning
Some Questions to Consider What makes a problem hard? Is there anything special about problems that seem to be solved in a flash of “insight”? How can analogies be used to help solve problems? How do experts in a field solve problems differently than nonexperts?
What Is a Problem? Obstacle between a present state and a goal Not immediately obvious how to get around the obstacle
What Is a Problem? Well-defined: correct answer, certain procedures will lead to solution Ill-defined: path to solution is unclear, no one “correct” answer
Gestalt Approach Representing a problem in the mind Restructuring: changes the problem’s representation Kohler’s “circle” problem
Caption: The circle problem.
Insight in Problem-Solving Sudden realization of a problem’s solution
Caption: (a) Triangle problem and (b) chain problem for “Two Insight Problems” demonstration.
Insight in Problem-Solving Metcalfe and Wiebe (1987) Insight: triangle problem, chain problem Noninsight: algebra Warmth judgments every 15 seconds
Insight in Problem-Solving Metcalfe and Wiebe (1987) Insight problems solved suddenly Noninsight problems solved gradually
Caption: Results of Metcalfe and Wiebe’s (1987) experiment showing how participants judged how close they were to solving insight problems and algebra problems for the minute just before solving the problems.
Obstacles to Problem-Solving Functional fixedness: restricting use of an object to its familiar functions Candle problem: seeing boxes as containers inhibited using them as supports Two-string problem: function of pliers gets in the way of seeing them as a weight
Obstacles to Problem-Solving Functional fixedness: the elevator riddle
Obstacles to Problem-Solving Situationally produced mental set Situation influences approach to problem Water-jug problem: given mental set inhibited participants from using simpler solution
Caption: Luchins’s (1942) water-jug problem Caption: Luchins’s (1942) water-jug problem. Each problem specifies the capacities of jugs A, B, and C, and a final desired quantity. The task is to use the jugs to measure out the final quantity. The solution to problem 1 is shown. All of the other problems can be solved using the same pattern of pourings, indicated by the equation, but there are more efficient ways to solve 7 and 8.
Information-Processing Approach Newell and Simon Problem space Initial state Intermediate state(s) Goal state
Information-Processing Approach Tower of Hanoi Operators: rules specify which moves are allowed and which are not
Figure 11. 1 Specialties in psychology. Percentages are approximate Figure 11.1 Specialties in psychology. Percentages are approximate. The percentages shown here are based on membership in the American Psychological Association. The APA welcomes both scientists and practitioners. However, some psychological researchers belong to other organizations, such as the American Psychological Society. As a result, the percentage of clinical and counseling psychologists working in the United States may be a little lower than shown in chart a. Nevertheless, it is accurate to say that most psychologists specialize in applied areas and work in applied settings. Caption: (a) Initial and goal states for the Tower of Hanoi problem. (b) The operators for the Tower of Hanoi problem.
Information-Processing Approach Means-end analysis: reduce differences between initial and goal states Subgoals: create intermediate states closer to goal
Caption: Initial steps in solving the Tower of Hanoi problem, showing how the problem can be broken down into subgoals.
The Importance of How a Problem Is Stated Acrobat and reverse acrobat problem One small change in wording of problem Not just analyzing structure of problem space How a problem is stated can affect its difficultly
The Importance of How a Problem Is Stated Mutilated-checkerboard problem Conditions differed in how much information provided about the squares Easier to solve when information is provided that points toward the correct representation of the problem
Caption: Conditions in Kaplan and Simon’s (1990) study of the mutilated-checkerboard problem. (Reprinted from Cognitive Psychology, Volume 22, C.A. Kaplan & H.A. Simon, “In Search of Insight,” pp. 374-419, Figure 2. Copyright © 1990, with permission from Elsevier.)
The Importance of How a Problem Is Stated Think-aloud protocol Say aloud what one is thinking Shift in how one perceives elements of a problem
Using Analogies to Solve a Problem Using a solution to a similar problem guides solution to new problem Russian marriage problem (source problem) mutilated-checkerboard problem (target problem)
Using Analogies to Solve a Problem Gick and Holyoak Noticing relationship Mapping correspondence between source and target Applying mapping
Using Analogies to Solve a Problem Duncker’s Radiation Problem Analogies aid problem-solving Often hints must be given to notice connection Surface features get in the way Structural features must be used
Caption: (a) Solution to the radiation problem Caption: (a) Solution to the radiation problem. Bombarding the tumor, in the center, with a number of low-intensity rays from different directions destroys the tumor without damaging the tissue it passes through. (b) Radiosurgery, a modern medical technique for irradiating brain tumors with a number of beams of gamma rays, uses the same principle. The actual technique uses 201 gamma ray beams. (c) How the general solved the fortress problem.
Using Analogies to Solve a Problem Lightbulb problem High surface similarities aid analogical problem solving Making structural features more obvious aids analogical problem-solving
Using Analogies to Solve a Problem Analogical encoding: comparing two cases that illustrate a principle Effective way to get participants to pay attention to structure features that aide problem-solving
Using Analogies to Solve a Problem Analogical paradox Participants in experiments focus on surface features People in the real world use structural features
Using Analogies to Solve a Problem In vivo problem-solving research People are observed to determine how they solve problems in the real world Advantage: naturalistic setting Disadvantages: time-consuming, cannot isolate and control variables
How Experts Solve Problems Experts solve problems in their field faster and with a higher success rate than beginners
How Experts Solve Problems Experts possess more knowledge about their fields
How Experts Solve Problems Knowledge is organized so it can be accessed when needed to work on a problem Novice: surface features Expert: deep structure
Caption: The kinds of physics problems that were grouped together by novices (left) and experts (right; Chi et al., 1981).
How Experts Solve Problems Experts spend more time analyzing problem Experts are no better than novices when given problems outside of their field
How Experts Solve Problems Experts less likely to be open to new ways of looking at problems
Creative Problem-Solving Creativity Innovative thinking Novel ideas New connections between existing ideas
Creative Problem-Solving Divergent thinking: open-ended; large number of potential “solutions” Convergent thinking: one correct answer
Creative Problem-Solving Design fixation Fixated on what not to do as demonstrated by sample Fixation can inhibit problem-solving
Creative Problem-Solving Creative cognition: technique to train people to think creatively Preinventive forms: ideas that precede creation of finished creative product
Caption: How a preinventive form that was constructed from the half-sphere, wire, and handle can be interpreted in terms of each of the eight categories in Table 11.1. (Reprinted from R. A. Finke, “Creative Insight and Preinventive Forms,” from The Nature of Insight, by R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson, Eds., pp. 255-280, Figure 8.6. Copyright © 1995 with permission from the MIT Press.