Common Ground Linguistic referents are established w/in a “domain of interpretation”, which includes context –One component of context = Common Ground.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Generation of Referring Expressions: the State of the Art LOT Winter School, Tilburg 2008 Kees van Deemter Computing Science University of Aberdeen.
Advertisements

Accessing spoken words: the importance of word onsets
Assessment Assessment should be an integral part of a unit of work and should support student learning. Assessment is the process of identifying, gathering.
Marslen-Wilson Big Question: “What processes take place during the period that the sensory information is accumulating for the listener” during spoken.
Eye Movements and Spoken Language Comprehension: effects of visual context on syntactic ambiguity resolution Spivey et al. (2002) Psych 526 Eun-Kyung Lee.
New Swannington Primary School EYFS Open Evening 2014.
What ’ s New? Acquiring New Information as a Process in Comprehension Suan E. Haviland & Herbert H. Clark.
Using prosody to avoid ambiguity: Effects of speaker awareness and referential context Snedeker and Trueswell (2003) Psych 526 Eun-Kyung Lee.
Readers routinely represent implied object rotation: The role of visual experience Wassenberg & Zwaan, in press, QJEP Brennan Payne Psych
Auditory Word Recognition
Understanding Pronouns Jennifer E. Arnold University of Pennsylvania.
Sentence Memory: A Constructive Versus Interpretive Approach Bransford, J.D., Barclay, J.R., & Franks, J.J.
Albert Gatt LIN1180 – Semantics Lecture 10. Part 1 (from last week) Theories of presupposition: the semantics- pragmatics interface.
XX/XX/XX Presenter names Position Title Accessibility “How to”
Pictures and spoken descriptions elicit similar eye movements during mental imagery, both in light and in complete darkness Roger Johansson, Jana Holsanova,
Consistency of Assessment
Organizational Notes no study guide no review session not sufficient to just read book and glance at lecture material midterm/final is considered hard.
What is Cognitive Science? … is the interdisciplinary study of mind and intelligence, embracing philosophy, psychology, artificial intelligence, neuroscience,
Conversation: Behavioral Foundations Stephanie Smale CPSC 781:CSCW.
What is Cognitive Science? … is the interdisciplinary study of mind and intelligence, embracing philosophy, psychology, artificial intelligence, neuroscience,
IMT530- Organization of Information Resources1 Feedback Like exercises –But want more instructions and feedback on them –Wondering about grading on these.
La Technologie des Mouvements Oculaires en Linguistique Expérimentale Rachel Shen.
Wilson, “The case for sensorimotor coding in working memory” Wilson’s thesis: Items held in short-term verbal memory are encoded in an “articulatory” format.
Announcements Next week: Seminar Next week: Seminar Read all 4 papers, but you are responsible for presenting certain ones. Read all 4 papers, but you.
Communication Ms. Morris.
Discrimination-Shift Problems Background This type of task has been used to compare concept learning across species as well as across a broad range of.
Stimulus Control.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Production & Comprehension: Conversation & Dialog.
Grammar-Translation Approach Direct Approach
ESL Phases & ESL Scale Curriculum Corporation 1994.
11.10 Human Computer Interface www. ICT-Teacher.com.
Various Definitions of Pragmatics. Morristhe study of the relations of signs to interpreters (1938) deals with the origin, uses, and effects of signs.
PRAGMATICS A: I have a fourteen year old son B: Well that's all right
What is Usability? Usability Is a measure of how easy it is to use something: –How easy will the use of the software be for a typical user to understand,
Name Workshop Facilitator Instructional Leadership: Creating Demand.
How do university students solve problems in vector calculus? Evidence from eye tracking Karolinska institutet Stockholm 4th of May 2012 Magnus Ögren 1.
A Collaboration between: Los Angeles Unified School District University of California, San Diego San Diego State University University of California, Irvine.
Strategy Stop QUICK Write Strategy: Springboard for Instructional Conversations Scaffolds: Bridging and Contextualization Language Domains: Writing and.
Expressing Yourself Effective Communication. Number your white board to 15.
Socratic Seminars EXPECTATIONS FOR A SUCCESSFUL DISCUSSION.
The Direct Method has one very basic rule: No translation is allowed.
Webinar Maths and SLCN Jean Gross CBE The webinar slides, plus additional strategies, can be downloaded.
Communicating Culture interviewing. Interviewing: Definition  Interviewing is a meeting of two persons to exchange information and ideas through questions.
Pragmatics.
Lectures ASSESSING LANGUAGE SKILLS Receptive Skills Productive Skills Criteria for selecting language sub skills Different Test Types & Test Requirements.
Input and Interaction Ellis (1985), interaction, as the discourse jointly constructed by the learner and his interlocutors and input is the result of.
The Effect of Interface on Social Action in Online Virtual Worlds Anthony Steed Department of Computer Science University College London.
Presentation about pragmatic concepts Implicatures Presuppositions
Speaking while monitoring addressees for understanding Torsten Jachmann Herbert H. Clark and Meredyth A. Krych Seminar „Gaze as function of.
Discourse and genre. What is a genre? A staged, goal-oriented, purposeful activity in which speakers engage as members of our culture (Martin, 1984: 25)
Welcome Back, Folks! We’re travelling to a littele bit far-end of Language in Use Studies EAA remains your faithful companion.
Which is the first language skill acquired in human Which is the first language skill acquired in human lives?
Chapter 8. Situated Dialogue Processing for Human-Robot Interaction in Cognitive Systems, Christensen et al. Course: Robots Learning from Humans Sabaleuski.
Introduction to Communicative Language Teaching Zhang Lu.
Developing Communication Skills Developing Listening Techniques.
Defining Discourse.
 And if negotiation and facilitation doesn’t work…  Or you haven’t had a chance to try it…  You may find yourself having to manage a conflict situation.
How we actively interpret our environment..  Perception: The process in which we understand sensory information.  Illusions are powerful examples of.
MEMORY TRAINING IN INTERPRETING NAYDA E. MEDINA D.
Developing coaching skills
Welcome! These slides are designed to help you think through presenting your evaluation planning and results. Feel free to pick and choose the slides that.
The Direct Method has one very basic rule: No translation is allowed.
Creating the Informative Speech
Approaches to Discourse Analysis
Helping Children Learn
The Components of the Phenomenon of Repetition Suppression
Deixis and distance  .
GRICE’S CONVERSATIONAL MAXIMS
On the PROCESSING of “might”
Language Functions.
Presentation transcript:

Common Ground Linguistic referents are established w/in a “domain of interpretation”, which includes context –One component of context = Common Ground Mutual knowledge, beliefs, & assumptions among participants in conversation Comes from community co-membership, physical co- presence, linguistic co-presence, …

Conversation & Common Ground Clark (1996) –Language = Joint action by people cooperating to achieve particular goals –Optimal communication requires keeping track of what’s in Common Ground and using that in both producing and understanding language As usual, what’s at issue is timing –How quickly do/can language users make use of knowledge about Common Ground when speaking & listening?

Keysar & Colleagues (1996, 2000) In several studies, find that listener’s immediate interpretations are “egocentric” –i.e., Don’t take into account which knowledge they share with speaker vs which knowledge only they have –Monitor eye movements to visual display containing referents for items mentioned in speech Speaker (confederate) gives listener spoken instructions to move objects in display Some objects visible to both (Common Ground) Some objects visible only to listener (Privileged Ground) Listeners just as likely to look at Privileged objects as at Common objects, if they match instructions best

Problem with Keysar’s Studies Very best physical match for instruction = object in Privileged Ground –“Put the bottom block below the apple.” When bottom-most block in listener’s display is visible only to listener Listeners just as likely to look at the bottom-most block in their display as to look at the block that they know is bottom-most in the speaker’s display Better to make the object in Common Ground just as good a match for the instructions as the object in Privileged Ground is

Another Potential Problem Common Ground is typically accumulated and adjusted incrementally over the course of conversation –In Keysar’s studies, listeners simply instructed about what was visible to whom Better to use an explicit grounding process with common ground established on the basis of linguistic co-presence

Hanna et al. (2003) – Experiment 1 Participants –12 Listeners (L) +Confederate Speaker (S) pairs –Listener told that Speaker was lab assistant but naïve about purpose of study (false) Apparatus = Head-mounted eyetracker –Eye camera tracks pupil using infrared light –Scene camera shows line of sight, so head can move & still know where eyes are looking –Spatial resolution = 1 degree –Temporal resolution = msec Sampling rate = 30 frames/sec

Experiment 1 Procedure Materials (more on next slide) –Boxes containing 3 x 3 grid of locations –For each trial, 7 objects each for S & L 5 are to be placed in the grid by L according to S’s instructions –S and L don’t see each other’s grids or objects Instructions –On each trial, get envelope with 7 objects + printed instructions –S’s instructions show layout of objects for trial + script of instructions to give Listener –L’s instructions include which of the objects is “secret” (= Privileged Ground) & where to put it in grid L knows S doesn’t know what shape is secret or where it is (true)

Visual Display Stimuli Target = Red triangle Competitor = Other triangle - Same or diff color from Target S = Secret Shape, seen by Listener only = Competitor or some other shape When Competitor = Secret, it’s in Privileged Ground - Will they look at it as much as they do to a Competitor that’s in Common Ground??? Competitor in Critical (= last) instruction from Speaker Competitor same color as Target “Competitor” diff color from Target

Same color competitor in Common Ground - L has to ask for clarification in this cond Same color competitor in Privileged Ground Fewer looks to Competitor in Privileged Ground by 400 msec after onset of Adj - Very similar to diff color conds

Hanna et al. (2002) – Expt 2 Possible criticism of Expt 1 (& other expts w/ similar design) –By design, Confederate Speaker doesn’t know about Privileged Ground objects & so never mentions them –Maybe Listeners move their eyes to objects that are more likely to be mentioned by Speaker Rather than taking Speaker’s perspective into account while interpreting referring expressions? Solution = Give Speaker & Listener different information about the state of some objects

Listener given 2 pairs of objects of same type –2 jars, 2 martini glasses, … –Objects described aloud by Exptr, left to right Sometimes objects described inaccurately –“2 empty jars”, when there is only 1 empty jar & 2 empty martini glasses –Listener told will be mistakes sometimes but not to talk with Speaker to correct them Told there’s another condition in Expt where Listeners get to make corrections Stimulus Displays & Descriptions

Instructions & Displays Display properties disambiguate instruction Early or Late –“Pick up the empty martini glass” –Late: 1 empty version of both types of objects –Early: 1 empty version of 1 type of object & 2 empty versions of other type of object Disambiguates early because definite “the empty” can have a unique ref only if only 1 of objects of a type is empty

Definite vs Indefinite Displays & Instructions “Pick up the empty martini glass.” vs “Pick up one of the empty martini glasses.” Late Early Late Early Mismatch Speaker told this But Listener sees this

Design Logic Listeners told to remember how objects were described to Speaker & to do what they think the Speaker intends them to do In Mismatch conditions, which type of object is the referent is disambiguated Early –But it’s a different object type for Speaker & Listener –Instruction: “Pick up the empty martini glass” Speaker believes: Only 1 empty martini glass & 2 empty jars Listener sees: 2 empty martini glasses & only 1 empty jar Questions –Will Listener look at glass she knows Speaker thinks empty? –If yes, how soon, compared to Early & Late Match conditions?

Results Late Early Mismatch

Conclusions Listeners look at target faster when instructions pick out a unique referent earlier –“the empty” when only 1 pair of objects has only 1 empty version Listeners can quickly take Speaker’s perspective into account –Look at target faster in Mismatch than in Late conditions From Speaker’s perspective, instruction picks out a unique referent early From Listener’s perspective, picks out other object type –But not as fast as in Early conditions So there’s some effort in taking Speaker’s perspective No evidence here for an initial egocentric stage