Highlights from the Program for the International Assessment of Adult Competencies (PIAAC), 2012 Jack Buckley National Center for Education Statistics.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Writing constructed response items
Advertisements

What can we learn from the international PISA study about improving reading at age 15? February 2011.
The good news, the not quite so good news and the not good news Jan Hagston –
The Primary Mathematics Curriculum in the UK with a particular focus on England Debbie Morgan Director for Primary Mathematics.
What is PIAAC?. About PIAAC PIAAC is an international large-scale assessment administered in in 23 countries It assessed 16 - to 65-year-olds,
OECD World Forum “Statistics, Knowledge and Policy”, Palermo, November Territorial Indicators for Regional Policies Vincenzo Spiezia Head,
The results from international assessments of adult literacy and numeracy skills Juliette Mendelovits CEET 17th Annual National Conference Friday 1 November.
Hunter centre for strathclyde Global Entrepreneurship Monitor Scotland 2002 Jonathan Levie Wendy Brown Laura Galloway.
Individuals at this level: can read brief texts on familiar topics and locate a single piece of specific information identical in form to information.
Adult Literacy and Lifeskills Survey This study was sponsored by the National Center for Education Statistics and the Employment and Training Administration.
PIAAC: OBJECTIVES AND METHODS New England Association of Colleges Conference, 2014 William Thorn, Senior Analyst, OECD
U.S. DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION Program for International Student Assessment (PISA) 2009 Results Stuart Kerachsky Deputy Commissioner December 7, 2010.
INTERNATIONAL LITERACY STATISTICS 2013 Presentation to the CUPE Literacy Working Group and the CUPE Learners’ Council Brigid Hayes, November 27, 2013.
Fall Webinar October 30, Open and hide your control panel Join audio: Choose “Mic & Speakers” to use VoIP Choose “Telephone” and dial using the.
Sondra Stein & Katie Landeros American Institutes for Research 1 Findings from the Program for the International Assessment of Adult Competencies (PIAAC),
International Survey of Adult Skills (ISAS) Policy Results 14th October 2013.
Slides developed by Sondra Stein, Katie Landeros,& Tim Werwath American Institutes for Research 1 Key Findings from the Program for the International Assessment.
PIAAC: ORIGINS, INTERNATIONAL DIMENSION, CONCEPTS AND AIMS William Thorn, OECD
Literacy for the 21 st Century – what will PIAAC mean for Ireland? 23 September 2013.
Overview of U.S. Results: Digital Problem Solving PIAAC results tell a story about the systemic nature of the skills deficit among U.S. adults.
Learning Policy Directorate, HRSDC 1 ALL 2003 Key Research Findings and HRSD Implications Presented by Satya Brink, Ph.D. Director, National Learning Policy.
PIAAC results tell a story about the systemic nature of the skills deficit among U.S. adults. Overview of U.S. Results: Focus on Numeracy.
An Examination of the Program for the International Assessment of Adult Competencies (PIAAC) Findings in the United States National Council for Workforce.
OECD Review of Russian Statistics Peer Review Mission to Russia April 2012 Tim Davis Head, Global Relations, Statistics Directorate.
1 U.S. PIAAC National Supplement: Prison Study Overview Association of State Correctional Administrators Research and Best Practices Committee Gaylord.
International Outcomes of Learning in Mathematics and Problem Solving: PISA 2003 Results from the U.S. Perspective Commissioner Robert Lerner National.
© 2006 Michigan State University, Center for Research in Mathematics and Science Education Content Standards in an International Context William H. Schmidt.
WJEC Psychology Psy 2 Core Studies
By: Victoria Macedo and Cody Carvahlo. To provide governments with a setting to discuss effective approaches to economic and social issues. Allows similar.
PISA2009 Results: our 21st century learners at age 15 6 December 2010 Maree Telford PISA 2009 National Project Manager.
1 Achievement, Standards, and Assessment in Iowa and in Iowa Districts.
Overview of U.S. Results: Focus on Literacy PIAAC results tell a story about the systemic nature of the skills deficit among U.S. adults.
1 PISA What is PISA?  International large-scale assessment organized by the Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD)  NCES.
2007 by The Education Trust, Inc. Program for International Student Assessment (PISA) 2006 Prepared by the Education Trust December 2007.
PIAAC: SOME THINGS IT TELLS US ABOUT HIGHER EDUCATION New England Association of Colleges Conference 2014 William Thorn Senior Analyst, OECD.
PISA OECD Programme for International Student Assessment Students On Line Students On Line Digital Technologies and Performance Programme for International.
PISA International Conference. Reading Performance of Hong Kong’s 15-Year-Old Students in PISA.
Capitalist. Main Points In a capitalist or free-market country, people can own their own businesses and property. People can also buy services for private.
Israel Accession Seminar PIAAC: Programme for International assessment of Adult Competencies Skills strategy in OECD Programme for the International Assessment.
International Comparison of Health Care Gene Chang.
1 United States Education at a Glance 2015 Andreas Schleicher Director for Education and Skills Release date: 24 November 2015.
Impact of the Crisis on Children in Europe Yekaterina Chzhen ChildONEurope Seminar Paris - November 26, 2015.
Results from the 2009 Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA): How does the United States compare to other nations? December 2010.
Problem gambling in Europe: Why a regulatory authority needed Dr Mark Griffiths Professor of Gambling Studies International Gaming Research Unit
NUMERACY “Well hey that’s just knowing numbers right.” NOT EXACTLY.
Statistical data on women entrepreneurs in Europe Jacqueline Snijders 11 October 2014.
© The Center for Public Education, 2009 What the international PIRLS test reveals about teaching second language learners A study of 4 th grade readers.
1 Perspectives on the Achievements of Irish 15-Year-Olds in the OECD PISA Assessment
1 Main achievement outcomes continued.... Performance on mathematics and reading (minor domains) in PISA 2006, including performance by gender Performance.
Best Sustainable Development Practices for Food Security UV-B radiation: A Specific Regulator of Plant Growth and Food Quality in a Changing Climate The.
1© GfK 2016 | Concerns about safety and security Concerns about safety and security Global GfK survey July 2016.
HEALTH INFORMATION SEEKING BEHAVIORS OF ADULTS WITH LOW LITERACY, NUMERACY, AND PROBLEM SOLVING SKILLS Exploring Results from the 2012 U.S. PIAAC Study.
NSO data collections of subjective well-being
PISA 2015 results in England
John Jerrim UCL Institute of Education
What is PIAAC?.
A First Look at the 2015 Program for International Student Assessment Financial Literacy Results Peggy G. Carr, Ph.D. Acting Commissioner Institute of.
Overview of U.S. Results: Focus on Literacy
Programme for International Student Assessment
PISA 2009 – New Approaches to Assessing Reading Literacy
Overview of U.S. Results: Focus on Numeracy
Global Housing Markets : A Supply Side View
How Canada Compares Internationally
Overview of U.S. Results: Focus on Numeracy
U.S. PIAAC Prison Study.
Jenny Bradshaw NCETM National CPD Conference 23rd March 2011
Developing Maths Improvement Practitioner Team.
Numeracy Achievement Gaps of Low- and High-Performing Adults: An Analysis Within and Across Countries David C. Miller, Ph.D. Belle Raim.
Describing and Interpreting graphs, charts and tables
Annual Epidemiological Report for 2017 Sexually Transmitted Diseases - chlamydia - gonorrhoea - lymphogranuloma venereum - (congenital) syphilis.
Presentation transcript:

Highlights from the Program for the International Assessment of Adult Competencies (PIAAC), 2012 Jack Buckley National Center for Education Statistics (NCES) Washington, DC October

What is PIAAC?  International large-scale assessment administered in in 23 countries  16- to 65-year-olds, non-institutionalized, residing in the country, irrespective of nationality, citizenship, or language status  Laptop computer or paper-and-pencil:  In the U.S., 80% took the computer tests and 15% took the paper-and-pencil tests.  Assessment subjects:  Literacy, Numeracy, and Problem Solving in Technology-Rich Environments  Conducted in English in the U.S.:  Background survey in English or Spanish. About 4% could not complete the questionnaire because of language difficulties or learning or mental disabilities, and 1% could not complete it for other reasons. 2

General patterns of U.S. results  Below international average in all subject areas  Ranked better in literacy than in numeracy or problem solving in technology-rich environments  Higher percentage at low proficiency levels than international average  Percentages of top performers similar to or slightly lower than international average, depending on the subject  Performance gap between young and older population smaller than the average gap internationally 3

4 Participating countries Australia Austria Belgium Canada Cyprus Czech Republic Denmark Estonia Finland France Germany Ireland Italy Japan Korea, Rep of Netherlands Norway Poland Slovak Republic Spain Sweden United Kingdom United States Chile Greece Indonesia Israel Lithuania New Zealand Singapore Slovenia Turkey

What PIAAC reports Average Scores: Reported on a scale of for all domains. Proficiency Levels: Reported as the percentages of adults scoring at six performance levels in literacy and numeracy and at four performance levels in problem solving in technology-rich environments. 5

Literacy proficiency levels Locate single piece of information in familiar texts. Read relatively short digital, print or mixed texts to locate single text. Make matches between text and information that may require low level para- phrasing and drawing low-level inferences. Identify, interpret, or evaluate one or more pieces of information and often require varying levels of inference. Perform multiple- step operations to integrate, interpret, or synthesize information from complex texts, and may require complex inferences. Integrate information across multiple, dense texts; construct syntheses, ideas or points of view; or evaluate evidence based arguments. Below Level 1 Level 1 Level 2 Level 3 Level 4 Level 5 6

Literacy example item Below Level 1: Election results  The stimulus is a report of the results of a union election. It consists of several brief paragraphs and a simple table identifying the three candidates and the number of votes they received.  The test taker is asked to identify which candidate received the fewest votes. To do this, the test taker must simply compare the number of votes that each candidate received.  The word “votes” appears only in the question and in the table. Therefore, the task consists of recognizing this direct relationship between the two to infer the answer. 7

Literacy example item Level 4: Library search  The stimulus displays the results of a bibliographic search from a simulated library website.  The test taker is asked to identify a book suggesting that the claims made both for and against genetically modified foods are unreliable. To do this, the test taker needs to read the title and description of each book included in the search results.  Many pieces of distracting information are present. The necessary information must be inferred from the statement that the author “describes how both sides in this hotly contested debate have manufactured propaganda, tried to dupe the public and... [text ends].” 8

Numeracy proficiency levels Perform basic tasks: counting, arithmetic operations with whole numbers. Perform one- step tasks: count; sort; arithmetic operations; understanding simple percent (ex. 50%). Perform 2 or more calculations, simple measurement; spatial representation; estimation; and interpret simple tables, graphs. Understand & work with mathematical patterns, proportions, basic statistics expressed in verbal or numerical form. Perform analysis, complex reasoning, statistics and chance; spatial relationships; and communicat- ing well- reasoned explanations for answers. Understand complex abstract mathema- tical and statistical ideas, embedded in complex texts, draw inferences; arguments or models; justify, reflect on solutions or choices. Below Level 1 Level 1 Level 2 Level 3 Level 4 Level 5 9

Numeracy example item Below Level 1: Price tag  The stimulus for this item consists of four supermarket price tags. The tags identify the product, the price per pound, the net weight, the date packed, and the total price.  The test taker is asked to indicate the item that was packed first by simply comparing the dates on the price tags. 10

Numeracy example item Level 4: Education level  The stimulus for this item consists of two stacked-column bar graphs presenting the distribution of the Mexican population by years of schooling for men and women separately.  The y axis of each graph is labeled “percentage” and includes grid lines for 0, 20, 40, 60, 80, and 100%. The x axis is labeled “year” and presents data for 1960, 1970, 1990, 2000, and A legend identifies three categories of schooling: “more than 6 years of schooling,” “up to 6 years of schooling,” and “no schooling.”  The test taker is asked to indicate what percentage of men in Mexico had more than 6 years of schooling in 1970, choosing from a pull-down menu that has 10 response categories: “0–10%,” “10– 20%,” and so on. 11

Problem solving in technology-rich environments proficiency levels Tasks are well- defined involving use of only one function within a generic interface. Tasks require little or no navigation, and only a few steps to access information for solving the problem. There are few monitoring demands. Tasks require some navigation across pages and applications for solving the problem. Evaluating the relevance, some integration and inferential reasoning may be needed. Task may involve multiple steps and operators, navigation across pages and applications. There are typically high monitoring demands, and evaluation of relevance and reliability of information. Below Level 1 Level 1 Level 2 Level 3 12

Problem Solving in technology-rich environments example item Level 1: Party invitations  This task involves sorting s into pre-existing folders in an inbox.  An interface is presented with five s in an inbox. The test taker is asked to place the s, which are responses to a party invitation, into folders to keep track of who can and cannot attend a party.  The task is performed in a single environment, and the goal is explicitly stated. Solving the problem requires a relatively small number of steps and does not demand a significant amount of monitoring across a large number of actions. 13

Problem Solving in technology-rich environments example item Level 3: Meeting rooms  This task involves managing requests to reserve a meeting room on a particular date using a reservation system.  The task presents two applications: an interface (with a number of s requesting reservations stored in an inbox) and a web-based reservation tool that allows the user to assign rooms to meetings at certain times.  Successfully completing the task involves taking into account multiple constraints (i.e., the number of rooms available and existing reservations). These constraints generate a conflict (one of the demands for a room reservation cannot be satisfied), which has to be resolved by issuing a standard message to decline one of the requests. 14

15 U.S. PIAAC Findings

U.S. average literacy score (270) lower than the international average (273) Lower than in 12 countries : Japan, Finland, Netherlands, Australia, Sweden, Norway, Estonia, Flanders-Belgium, Czech Republic, Slovak Republic, Canada, Republic of Korea Not significantly different than in 5 countries : England and Northern Ireland- U.K., Denmark, Germany, Austria, Cyprus Higher than in 5 countries : Poland, Ireland, France, Spain, Italy 16

17 Below level 1 range: Level 1 range: Level 2 range: Level 3 range: Level 4/5 range: Median Seven countries had higher percentages of adults reaching the highest proficiency level (4/5) in literacy Median

Higher proportion of U.S. adults at the bottom levels of literacy 18

Lower than in 12 countries: Japan, Finland, Netherlands, Republic of Korea, Estonia, Flanders- Belgium, Australia, Sweden, Poland, Czech Republic, Germany, Austria Not significantly different than in 8 countries: Denmark, Slovak Republic, Canada, Norway, France, Ireland, Cyprus, England and Northern Ireland- U.K. Higher than in 2 countries: Spain, Italy U.S. 16- to 24-year-olds below international average in literacy, rank lower than 16- to 65-year-olds overall 19

Only oldest U.S. adults outperformed the international average in literacy 20 *p <.05. U.S. average score is significantly different from PIAAC international average.

Least educated adults below the international average in literacy 21 *p <.05. U.S. average score is significantly different from PIAAC international average.

Employed adults in the U.S. had lower average literacy scores than their peers internationally 22 *p <.05. U.S. average score is significantly different from PIAAC international average.

U.S. White adults had higher average literacy scores than either Black or Hispanic adults 23 *p <.05. Average score is significantly different from White average.

24 U.S. gaps in literacy scores larger than international average by parental education and nativity status

25 In literacy, U.S. gaps larger by educational attainment and skill level of job, but similar to international average by income and employment status

U.S. gaps in literacy scores similar to international average by gender, smaller by age, and larger by health status 26

U.S. average literacy score in 2012 not significantly different from 2003, but lower than in *p <.05. Average score is significantly different from PIAAC.

Summary of literacy findings:  Lower overall literacy scores than international average  Higher percentage of low performers than international average  Gaps between less advantaged socio-economic groups and more advantaged peers higher in U.S. than internationally  No change in overall U.S. literacy scores since

U.S. average numeracy score (253) lower than the international average (269) Lower than in 18 countries: Japan, Finland, Flanders-Belgium, Netherlands, Sweden, Norway, Denmark, Slovak Republic, Czech Republic, Austria, Estonia, Germany, Australia, Canada, Cyprus, Republic of Korea, England and Northern Ireland- U.K., Poland Not significantly different than in 2 countries: Ireland, France Higher than in 2 countries: Italy, Spain 29

Fifteen countries had higher percentages of adults reaching the highest proficiency level (4/5) in numeracy 30 Below level 1 range: Level 1 range: Level 2 range: Level 3 range: Level 4/5 range: Median

31 Higher proportion of U.S. adults at the bottom levels of numeracy

Lower than in 21 countries : Netherlands, Finland, Japan, Flanders-Belgium, Republic of Korea, Austria, Estonia, Sweden, Czech Republic, Slovak Republic, Germany, Denmark, Norway, Australia, Poland, Canada, Cyprus, France, Ireland, England and Northern Ireland- U.K., Spain Not significantly different than in 1 country: Italy U.S. 16- to 24-year-olds below international average in numeracy, rank lower than 16- to 65-year-olds overall 32

U.S. adults in all age groups below international average in numeracy 33 *p <.05. U.S. average score is significantly different from PIAAC international average.

U.S. adults at every education level below the international average in numeracy 34 *p <.05. U.S. average score is significantly different from PIAAC international average.

Employed adults in the U.S. had lower average numeracy scores than their peers internationally 35 *p <.05. U.S. average score is significantly different from PIAAC international average.

U.S. White adults had higher average numeracy scores than either Black or Hispanic adults 36 *p <.05. Average score is significantly different from White average.

U.S. gaps in numeracy scores larger than international average by parental education, not different by nativity status 37

38 In numeracy, U.S. gaps larger by educational attainment, income, and skill level of job, but similar to international average by employment status

39 In numeracy, U.S. gap similar to international average by gender, smaller by age, and larger by health status

U.S. average numeracy score in 2012 lower than in *p <.05. Average score is significantly different from PIAAC.

Summary of numeracy findings:  Lower overall numeracy scores than international average  Higher percentages of low performers than international average  Regardless of educational level or gender, U.S. adults lower than international average  Lower U.S. numeracy scores than in

U.S. average problem solving in technology-rich environments score (277) lower than the international average (283) Lower than in 14 countries : Japan, Finland, Australia, Sweden, Norway, Netherlands, Austria, Denmark, Czech Republic, Republic of Korea, Germany, Canada, Slovak Republic, Flanders-Belgium Not significantly different than in 4 countries: England and Northern Ireland- U.K., Estonia, Ireland, Poland 42

Eight countries had higher percentages reaching the highest proficiency level (3) in problem solving in technology-rich environments 43 Below level 1 range: Level 1 range: Level 2 range: Level 3 range: Median

44 Higher proportion of U.S. adults at the bottom levels of problem solving in technology-rich environments

Lower than in 14 countries: Republic of Korea, Finland, Sweden, Netherlands, Japan, Flanders-Belgium, Czech Republic, Norway, Australia, Germany, Austria, Canada, Denmark, Estonia Not significantly different than in 4 countries: England and Northern Ireland- U.K., Slovak Republic, Poland, Ireland U.S. 16- to 24-year-olds below international average in problem solving in technology-rich environments 45

Only oldest U.S. adults outperformed the international average in problem solving in technology-rich environments 46 *p <.05. U.S. average scores are significantly different from PIAAC international average.

Least educated adults below the international average in problem solving in technology-rich environments 47 *p <.05. U.S. average scores are significantly different from PIAAC international average.

Employed adults in the U.S. had lower average problem solving in technology-rich environments scores than their peers internationally 48 *p <.05. U.S. average scores are significantly different from PIAAC international average.

U.S. White adults had higher average problem solving in technology-rich environments scores than either Black or Hispanic adults 49 *p <.05. Average scores are significantly different from White average.

50 In problem solving in technology-rich environments, U.S. gaps similar to international average by educational attainment, income, employment status, and skill level of job

51 In problem solving in technology-rich environments, U.S. gaps similar to international average by gender and health status, but smaller by age

Summary of problem solving in technology-rich environments findings:  Lower overall problem solving in technology- rich environments scores than international average  Higher percentages of low performers than international average  Gaps between youngest and oldest age groups smaller in U.S. than internationally 52

U.S. PIAAC Findings Summary: 53 U.S. PIAAC Findings Summary:  Lower overall scores than international average in all subjects  Higher percentages of low performers than internationally  Larger gaps between less advantaged and more advantaged peers in literacy and numeracy, but not in problem solving in technology-rich environments  Relatively lower performance of young adults and those with high school education or less  Relatively higher performance of older adults in literacy and problem solving in technology-rich environments

54

For more information Contact: Eugene Owen NCES PIAAC at NCES: ‎ 55