Biostatistics: An Introduction RISE Program 2010 Los Angeles Biomedical Research Institute at Harbor-UCLA Medical Center January 15, 2010 Peter D. Christenson.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
1 COMM 301: Empirical Research in Communication Lecture 15 – Hypothesis Testing Kwan M Lee.
Advertisements

Inference Sampling distributions Hypothesis testing.
Estimation of Sample Size
+ Chapter 10 Section 10.4 Part 2 – Inference as Decision.
Inference: Confidence Intervals
Chapter 10: Hypothesis Testing
SAMPLE SIZE ESTIMATION
EPIDEMIOLOGY AND BIOSTATISTICS DEPT Esimating Population Value with Hypothesis Testing.
Evaluating Hypotheses Chapter 9. Descriptive vs. Inferential Statistics n Descriptive l quantitative descriptions of characteristics.
Introduction to Inference Estimating with Confidence Chapter 6.1.
Lecture 2: Thu, Jan 16 Hypothesis Testing – Introduction (Ch 11)
PSY 1950 Confidence and Power December, Requisite Quote “The picturing of data allows us to be sensitive not only to the multiple hypotheses that.
Copyright © 2010 Pearson Education, Inc. Chapter 19 Confidence Intervals for Proportions.
Inferences About Process Quality
Chapter 9 Hypothesis Testing.
Statistics for Health Care
Sample Size Determination
Sample size and study design
Chapter 19: Confidence Intervals for Proportions
McGraw-Hill/IrwinCopyright © 2009 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. All Rights Reserved. Chapter 9 Hypothesis Testing.
Hypothesis Testing.
Dr Mohammad Hossein Fallahzade Determining the Size of a Sample In the name of God.
Statistical Analysis Statistical Analysis
14. Introduction to inference
Biostatistics in Clinical Research Peter D. Christenson Biostatistician January 12, 2005IMSD U*STAR RISE.
Biostatistics for Coordinators Peter D. Christenson REI and GCRC Biostatistician GCRC Lecture Series: Strategies for Successful Clinical Trials Session.
Inference for a Single Population Proportion (p).
Biostatistics Case Studies 2015 Youngju Pak, PhD. Biostatistician Session 2: Sample Size & Power for Inequality and Equivalence Studies.
CHAPTER 16: Inference in Practice. Chapter 16 Concepts 2  Conditions for Inference in Practice  Cautions About Confidence Intervals  Cautions About.
Jan 17,  Hypothesis, Null hypothesis Research question Null is the hypothesis of “no relationship”  Normal Distribution Bell curve Standard normal.
Statistical Power and Sample Size Calculations Drug Development Statistics & Data Management July 2014 Cathryn Lewis Professor of Genetic Epidemiology.
Chapter 8 Introduction to Hypothesis Testing
Statistics for Health Care Biostatistics. Phases of a Full Clinical Trial Phase I – the trial takes place after the development of a therapy and is designed.
Psy B07 Chapter 4Slide 1 SAMPLING DISTRIBUTIONS AND HYPOTHESIS TESTING.
Biostatistics: Study Design Peter D. Christenson Biostatistician Summer Fellowship Program July 2, 2004.
10.2 Tests of Significance Use confidence intervals when the goal is to estimate the population parameter If the goal is to.
Lecture 16 Dustin Lueker.  Charlie claims that the average commute of his coworkers is 15 miles. Stu believes it is greater than that so he decides to.
1 Chapter 10: Introduction to Inference. 2 Inference Inference is the statistical process by which we use information collected from a sample to infer.
Statistical Principles for Clinical Research Sponsored by: NIH General Clinical Research Center Los Angeles Biomedical Research Institute at Harbor-UCLA.
Biostatistics in Practice Peter D. Christenson Biostatistician LABioMed.org /Biostat Session 4: Study Size and Power.
Lecture 16 Section 8.1 Objectives: Testing Statistical Hypotheses − Stating hypotheses statements − Type I and II errors − Conducting a hypothesis test.
Biostatistics in Practice Peter D. Christenson Biostatistician Session 4: Study Size and Power.
Chapter 221 What Is a Test of Significance?. Chapter 222 Thought Question 1 The defendant in a court case is either guilty or innocent. Which of these.
Economics 173 Business Statistics Lecture 4 Fall, 2001 Professor J. Petry
Biostatistics in Practice Peter D. Christenson Biostatistician Session 6: Case Study.
Statistics in Biomedical Research RISE Program 2011 Los Angeles Biomedical Research Institute at Harbor-UCLA Medical Center January 13, 2011 Peter D. Christenson.
6.1 Inference for a Single Proportion  Statistical confidence  Confidence intervals  How confidence intervals behave.
Statistics in Biomedical Research RISE Program 2012 Los Angeles Biomedical Research Institute at Harbor-UCLA Medical Center January 19, 2012 Peter D. Christenson.
Ch 10 – Intro To Inference 10.1: Estimating with Confidence 10.2 Tests of Significance 10.3 Making Sense of Statistical Significance 10.4 Inference as.
Introduction to Inference: Confidence Intervals and Hypothesis Testing Presentation 4 First Part.
Biostatistics in Practice Peter D. Christenson Biostatistician Session 3: Testing Hypotheses.
Fall 2002Biostat Statistical Inference - Confidence Intervals General (1 -  ) Confidence Intervals: a random interval that will include a fixed.
Biostatistics in Practice Peter D. Christenson Biostatistician Session 4: Study Size for Precision or Power.
Review I A student researcher obtains a random sample of UMD students and finds that 55% report using an illegally obtained stimulant to study in the past.
Biostatistics Case Studies 2006 Peter D. Christenson Biostatistician Session 1: Demonstrating Equivalence of Active Treatments:
© Copyright McGraw-Hill 2004
Copyright © 2013, 2009, and 2007, Pearson Education, Inc. Chapter 10 Comparing Two Groups Section 10.1 Categorical Response: Comparing Two Proportions.
Biostatistics in Practice Peter D. Christenson Biostatistician LABioMed.org /Biostat Session 3: Testing Hypotheses.
SAMPLING DISTRIBUTION OF MEANS & PROPORTIONS. SAMPLING AND SAMPLING VARIATION Sample Knowledge of students No. of red blood cells in a person Length of.
Copyright © 2010, 2007, 2004 Pearson Education, Inc. Chapter 19 Confidence Intervals for Proportions.
Uncertainty and confidence Although the sample mean,, is a unique number for any particular sample, if you pick a different sample you will probably get.
Biostatistics Case Studies 2006 Peter D. Christenson Biostatistician Session 1: Demonstrating Equivalence of Active Treatments:
Copyright © 2010 Pearson Education, Inc. Slide
Hypothesis Tests for 1-Proportion Presentation 9.
Statistics 19 Confidence Intervals for Proportions.
Chapter 9 Introduction to the t Statistic
Inference for a Single Population Proportion (p)
Biostatistics Case Studies 2007
Objectives 6.1 Estimating with confidence Statistical confidence
Objectives 6.1 Estimating with confidence Statistical confidence
Presentation transcript:

Biostatistics: An Introduction RISE Program 2010 Los Angeles Biomedical Research Institute at Harbor-UCLA Medical Center January 15, 2010 Peter D. Christenson

Scientific Decision Making Setting: Two groups, one gets drug A, one gets placebo (B). Measure outcome. How do we decide if the drug has an effect? Perhaps: Say yes if the mean outcome of those receiving drug is greater than the mean of the others?

Outline Meaning or randomness? Decisions, truth and errors. Sensitivity and specificity. Laws of large numbers. Experiment size and study power. Study design considerations. Resources, software, and references.

Meaning or Randomness?

This is the goal of science in general. The role of statistics is to give an objective way to make those decisions.

Meaning or Randomness? Scientific inferential statistics: Make a decision: Is it real or random? Quantify chances that the decision is correct or not. Other arenas of life: Suspect guilty? Nobel laureate's opinion? Make a decision: Is it real or random? Cannot quantify. Suppose something “remarkable” happens.

Decision Making We first discuss using a medical device to make decisions about a patient. These decisions could be correct or wrong. We then make an analogy to using an experiment to make decisions about a scientific question. These decisions could be correct or wrong.

Decision Making: Diagnosis Mammogram Spot Darkness 100 Definitely Not Cancer Definitely Cancer How is the decision made for intermediate darkness?

Decision Making: Diagnosis Mammogram Spot Darkness 100 Suppose a study found the mammogram rating (0-10) for 1000 women who definitely have cancer by biopsy (truth). Proportion of 1000 Women: 1000/1000 0/ / / / /

Decision Making: Sensitivity Cutoff for Spot DarknessMammogram Sensitivity ≥0≥0100% >299% >490% >660% >810% >100% Sensitivity = Chances of correctly detecting disease. Why not just choose a low cutoff and detect almost everyone with disease?

Decision Making Continued Mammogram Spot Darkness 100 Suppose a study found the mammogram rating (0-10) for 1000 women who definitely do NOT have cancer by biopsy (truth). Proportion of 1000 Women: 1000/1000 0/ / / / /

Decision Making: Specificity Cutoff for Spot DarknessMammogram Specificity <0<00% ≤2≤235% ≤4≤470% ≤6≤690% ≤8≤895% ≤10100% Specificity=Chances of correctly NOT detecting disease.

Decision Making: Tradeoff CutoffSensitivitySpecificity 0100%0% 299%35% 490%70% 660%90% 810%95% 100%100% Choice of cutoff depends on whether the diagnosis is a screening or a final one. For example: Cutoff=4 : Call disease in 90% with it and 30% without.

Make Decision: If Spot>6, Decide CA. If Spot≤6, Decide Not CA. True Non-CA Patients True CA Patients Mammogram Spot Darkness \\\ = Specificity = 90%. /// = Sensitivity = 60%. Graphical Representation of Tradeoffs Area under curve = Probability

Decision Making for Diagnosis: Summary As sensitivity increases, specificity decreases and vice-versa. Cannot increase both sensitivity and specificity together. We now develop sensitivity and specificity to test or decide scientific claims. True Disease ↔ True claim, real effect. Decide Disease ↔ Decide claim is true, effect is real. But, can both increase sensitivity and specificity together.

Scientific Decision Making Setting: Two groups, one gets drug A, one gets placebo (B). Measure outcome. How do we decide if the drug has an effect? Perhaps: Say yes if the mean of those receiving drug is greater than the mean of the others? Let’s work backwards: Start with what we want.

Make Decision: If Δ>2, then Decide A≠B. If Δ≤2, then Decide A=B. True No Effect (A=B) True Effect (A≈B+2.2) Eventual Graphical Representation 1. Where do these curves come from? 2. What are the consequences of using cutoff=2? Δ = Group A Mean minus Group B Mean

Question 2 First 2. What are the consequences of using cutoff=2? Answer: If the effect is real (A≠B), there is a 60% chance of deciding so. [Actually, if specifically A is 2.2 more than B.] This is the experiment’s sensitivity, more often called power. If effect is not real (A=B), so that A≠B just due to randomness in this study, there is a 90% of deciding so. This is the experiment’s specificity. More often, 100-specificity is called the level of significance.

Question 2 Continued What if cutoff=1 was used instead? Answer: If the effect is real (A≠B), there is about a 85% chance of deciding so. Sensitivity ↑ (from 60%). If effect is not real (A=B), so that A≠B just due to randomness in this study, there is about a 60% of deciding so. Specificity ↓ (from about 90%).

Typical Choice of Cutoff Δ = Group A Mean minus Group B Mean Require specificity to be 95%. This means there is only a 5% chance of wrongly declaring an effect. → Need overwhelming evidence, beyond a reasonable (5%) doubt, to make a claim. ~45% Power 95% Specificity

Power of the Scientific Method Scientists (and their journals and FDA) require overwhelming evidence, beyond a reasonable (5%) doubt, not just “preponderance of the truth” which would be specificity=50%. Similar to US court of law. So much stronger than expert opinion. ~45% Power Only 5% chance of a false positive claim How can we increase power, but maintain the chances of a false positive conclusion at ≤5%? Are we just stuck with knowing that many true conjectures will be thrown away as collateral damage to this rigor?

Back to Question 1 1.Where do the curves in the last figure come from? Answer: You specify three quantities: (1) where their peaks are (the experiment’s detectable difference), and how wide they are (determined by (2) natural variation and (3) the # of subjects or animals or tissue samples, N). Those specifications give a unique set of “bell- shaped” curves. How?

A “Law of Large Numbers” Suppose individuals have values ranging from Lo to Hi, but the % with any particular value could be anything, say: You choose a sample of 2 of these individuals, and find their average. What value do you expect the average to have? Lo Hi Prob ↑ N = 1

A “Law of Large Numbers” In both cases, values near the center will be more likely: Now choose a sample of 4 of these individuals, and find their average. What value do you expect the average to have? Lo Hi Prob ↑ N = 2

A “Law of Large Numbers” In both cases, values near the center will be more likely: Now choose a sample of 10 of these individuals, and find their average. What value do you expect the average to have? Lo Hi Prob ↑ N = 4

A “Law of Large Numbers” In both cases, values near the center will be more likely: Now choose a sample of 50 of these individuals, and find their average. What value do you expect the average to have? Lo Hi Prob ↑ N = 10

A “Law of Large Numbers” In both cases, values near the center will be more likely: A remarkable fact is that not only is the mean of the sample is expected to be close to the mean of “everyone” if N is large enough, but we know exact probabilities of how close, and the shape of the curve. Lo Hi Prob ↑ N = 50

Summary: Law of Large Numbers Lo Hi Prob ↑ N = 1 SD ↔ ↔ Lo Value of mean of N Hi ↔ SD(Mean) = SD/√N SD is about 1/6 of the total range. SD ≈ 1.25 x average deviation from the center. Large N

Scientific Decision Making So, where are we? We can now answer the basic dilemma we raised. Repeat earlier slide:

The Power of the Scientific Method Scientists (and their journals and FDA) require overwhelming evidence, beyond a reasonable (5%) doubt, not just “preponderance of the truth” which would be specificity=50%. Similar to US court of law. So much stronger than expert opinion. ~45% Power Only 5% chance of a false positive claim How can we increase power, but maintain the chances of a false positive conclusion at ≤5%? Are we just stuck with knowing that many true conjectures will be thrown away as collateral damage to this rigor? N ≈ 170

Scientific Decision Making So, the answer to the basic dilemma is that by choosing N large enough, we get more precise means, which narrow the curves, which increases the study power. We can thus find true effects with high certainty, say 80%, and also not claim false effects very often (5%): Only 5% chance of a false positive claim 80% Power N ≈ 380

In many experiments, five factors are inter-related. Specifying four of these determines the fifth: 1.Study size, N. 2.Power, usually 80% to 90% is used. 3.Acceptable false positive chance, usually 5%. 4.Magnitude of the effect to be detected (Δ). 5.Heterogeneity among subjects or units (SD). The next 2 slides show how these factors are typically examined, and easy software to do the calculations. Putting it All Together

Quote from An LA BioMed Protocol Thus, with a total of the planned 80 subjects, we are 80% sure to detect (p<0.05) group differences if treatments actually differ by at least 5.2 mm Hg in MAP change, or by a mean 0.34 change in number of vasopressors.

Software for Previous Slide Pilot data: SD=8.19 for ΔMAP in 36 subjects. For p-value<0.05, power=80%, N=40/group, the detectable Δ of 5.2 in the previous table is found as:

Study Size : May Not be Based on Power Precision refers to how well a measure is estimated. Margin of error = the ± value (half-width) of the 95% confidence interval (sorry – not discussed here). Smaller margin of error ←→ greater precision. To achieve a specified margin of error, solve the CI formula for N. Polls: N ≈ 1000→ margin of error on % ≈ 1/√N ≈ 3%. Pilot Studies, Phase I, Some Phase II: Power not relevant; may have a goal of obtaining an SD for future studies.

Study Design Considerations Statistical Components of Protocols Target population / source of subjects. Quantification of aims, hypotheses. Case definitions, endpoints quantified. Randomization plan, if one will be used. Masking, if used. Study size: screen, enroll, complete. Use of data from non-completers. Justification of study size (power, precision, other). Methods of analysis. Mid-study analyses.

Resources, Software, and References

Professional Statistics Software Package Output Enter code; syntax. Stored data; access- ible. Comprehensive, but steep learning curve: SAS, SPSS, Stata.

Microsoft Excel for Statistics Primarily for descriptive statistics. Limited output.

Typical Statistics Software Package Select Methods from Menus Output after menu selection Data in spreadsheet $100 - $500

Free Statistics Software: Mystat

Free Study Size Software

This and other biostat talks posted

Recommended Textbook: Making Inference Design issues Biases How to read papers Meta-analyses Dropouts Non-mathematical Many examples

Thank You Nils Simonson, in Furberg & Furberg, Evaluating Clinical Research