What readings does a given sentence have? Some sentences containing a quantifier and negation are semantically ambiguous. They reveal two readings: Alle.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Project B1 (von Heusinger, Brandtner) The interpretation of derived nominals.
Advertisements

A Comparison of Three Language Assessment Tools
Grammar: Meaning and Contexts * From Presentation at NCTE annual conference in Pittsburgh, 2005.
1 1 Chapter 5: Multiple Regression 5.1 Fitting a Multiple Regression Model 5.2 Fitting a Multiple Regression Model with Interactions 5.3 Generating and.
Eye Movements and Spoken Language Comprehension: effects of visual context on syntactic ambiguity resolution Spivey et al. (2002) Psych 526 Eun-Kyung Lee.
Why study grammar? Knowledge of grammar facilitates language learning
SEMANTICS.
Approaches to Learning and Social Identity: Attracting Mature Students into Higher Education Chris Howard and Peter Davies Chris Howard and Peter Davies.
Understanding the Research Process
Statistical NLP: Lecture 3
Nuclear Accent Shape and the Perception of Prominence Rachael-Anne Knight Prosody and Pragmatics 15 th November 2003.
The truth shall make you slow: Superlative Quantifiers as speech act modifiers Aviya Hacohen, Dana Kozlowski & Ariel Cohen Ben-Gurion University of the.
Sentence Memory: A Constructive Versus Interpretive Approach Bransford, J.D., Barclay, J.R., & Franks, J.J.
THEORY OF KNOWLEDGE and SCHOOL ECONOMICS AND BUSINESS Whose Perspectives? and other Problems of Knowledge.
January 12, Statistical NLP: Lecture 2 Introduction to Statistical NLP.
INTRODUCTION TO TRANSLATION STUDIES
Language, Mind, and Brain by Ewa Dabrowska Chapter 2: Language processing: speed and flexibility.
Input-Output Relations in Syntactic Development Reflected in Large Corpora Anat Ninio The Hebrew University, Jerusalem The 2009 Biennial Meeting of SRCD,
Linguisitics Levels of description. Speech and language Language as communication Speech vs. text –Speech primary –Text is derived –Text is not “written.
C SC 620 Advanced Topics in Natural Language Processing 3/9 Lecture 14.
PRAGMATICS. 3- Pragmatics is the study of how more gets communicated than is said. It explores how a great deal of what is unsaid is recognized. 4.
Research Methods Chapter 1. Behavioral Research Behavioral Medicine Communication Criminology Human Development Education Psychology Sociology.
 There are times when an experiment cannot be carried out, but researchers would like to understand possible relationships in the data. Data is collected.
How do you find square roots? How do you estimate square roots?
Introduction to Research Methodology
© 2013 Cengage Learning. Outline  Types of Cross-Cultural Research  Method validation studies  Indigenous cultural studies  Cross-cultural comparisons.
Thinking Like a Modern Economist 6 Economics is what economists do. — Jacob Viner CHAPTER 6 Copyright © 2010 by the McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. All rights.
Karina SZASZVARI Institute of Psychology, University of Pécs, Hungary.
LEARNING PROGRAMME Hypothesis testing Intermediate Training in Quantitative Analysis Bangkok November 2007.
Measuring Hint Level in Open Cloze Questions Juan Pino, Maxine Eskenazi Language Technologies Institute Carnegie Mellon University International Florida.
Experimental study of morphological priming: evidence from Russian verbal inflection Tatiana Svistunova Elizaveta Gazeeva Tatiana Chernigovskaya St. Petersburg.
The Psychology of the Person Chapter 2 Research Naomi Wagner, Ph.D Lecture Outlines Based on Burger, 8 th edition.
Word order and tonal shape in the production of focus in short Finnish utterances Martti Vainio 1, Juhani Järvikivi 2 and Stefan Werner 3 1 University.
THE BIG PICTURE Basic Assumptions Linguistics is the empirical science that studies language (or linguistic behavior) Linguistics proposes theories (models)
Language acquisition and language learning: the linguistic background of language development Marina Tzakosta LaDiva coordinator University of Crete Dept.
Chapter 2 Doing Social Psychology Research. Why Should You Learn About Research Methods?  It can improve your reasoning about real-life events  This.
1 Chapter 4 Syntax The sentence patterns of language Part I.
E CON 432--C HAPTER 2 Tools of Positive Analysis.
1 LIN 1310B Introduction to Linguistics Prof: Nikolay Slavkov TA: Qinghua Tang CLASS 24, April 3, 2007.
The Silent Way. Outline: Peculiarities Language theory Silence Principles Teaching materials.
Linguistic Essentials
Background: Speakers use prosody to distinguish between the meanings of ambiguous syntactic structures (Snedeker & Trueswell, 2004). Discourse also has.
Data Analysis Econ 176, Fall Populations When we run an experiment, we are always measuring an outcome, x. We say that an outcome belongs to some.
Cognitive Processes PSY 334 Chapter 11 – Language Structure June 2, 2003.
E BERHARD- K ARLS- U NIVERSITÄT T ÜBINGEN SFB 441 Coordinate Structures: On the Relationship between Parsing Preferences and Corpus Frequencies Ilona Steiner.
Literacy Processes Dr. Linda Wedwick & Dr. Darci J. Harland.
Copyright © Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.
Chapter 8: Simple Linear Regression Yang Zhenlin.
3 Phonology: Speech Sounds as a System No language has all the speech sounds possible in human languages; each language contains a selection of the possible.
Defining Discourse.
SIMS 296a-4 Text Data Mining Marti Hearst UC Berkeley SIMS.
Lecture 10 Semantics Sentence Interpretation. The positioning of words and phrases in syntactic structure helps determine the meaning of the entire sentence.
Lexical, Prosodic, and Syntactics Cues for Dialog Acts.
Discourse Analysis Week 10 Riggenbach (1999) Chapter 1 - Quotes.
Describe the Role of Situational and Dispositional Factors in Explaining Behaviour By Mr Daniel Hansson.
Copyright © 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. Chapter 11 Understanding Randomness.
Data Analysis. Qualitative vs. Quantitative Data collection methods can be roughly divided into two groups. It is essential to understand the difference.
Oral Corrective Feedback in Second Language Classrooms
V v Changes in Acceptance of Diversity through Volunteering with Youth with Disabilities COLLEGE OF PUBLIC HEALTH AND HUMAN SCIENCES Introduction: The.
Labov’s Principles—1972 Language in Society, Vol.1 No. 1 “ Principles ” 1.Cumulative Principle 2.The Neogrammarian Hypothesis 3.The Uniformitarian Principle.
Job Satisfaction and Subjective Well-Being: Test of a Moderated Mediational Model Serdar Karabati, Ph.D., Istanbul Bilgi University & Nurcan Ensari, Ph.D.,
Strengths and limitations for non-native speaker English teachers in teaching practice Yi Zhang.
Syntax 1 Introduction.
Statistical NLP: Lecture 3
What is linguistics?.
Studying Intonation Julia Hirschberg CS /21/2018.
Saidna Zulfiqar bin Tahir STATE UNIVERSITY OF MAKASSAR
Comparative Studies Avesani et al 1995; Hirschberg&Avesani 1997
Linguistic Essentials
Competence and performance
Presentation transcript:

What readings does a given sentence have? Some sentences containing a quantifier and negation are semantically ambiguous. They reveal two readings: Alle Politiker sind nicht korrupt. This will become clear if you consider the two different intonation contours : the I(ntonation)- Topicalization in (1a), versus focus in (1b). (1a) (1b) Background In semantic research judgements based on intuitions are used as primary data. But can we trust our intuitions? Is our “real” linguistic behavior different from what we think about it from a meta-perspective? Implications As the above example shows experimental methods can provide a more reliable basis for semantic research. Some advantages of an experimental approach are: semanticists have an explicit theory about what the judgements should be, subjects don‘t  the “labor” is divided between the subject and the experimenter/theorist so that judgements become easier many variables can be controlled (Lexis: frequency, length, phonotactics; syntactic structure; pragmatic factors) judgments about many similar sentences  generalizations about the construction itself judgements from a large number of informants  generalizations to “all speakers” the data can be analyzed using statistical methods A possible solution: Eliciting judgements using experimental methods 10 subjects took part in a perception pilot study. 5 sentences like (3) were presented auditorially to the subjects, and they had to rate how well these sentences fit with contexts that disambiguated the scope. For rating the magnitude estimation method (Bard et al., 1996) was used. Predictions: Büring predicts that (4) and (5) should exhibit both readings. Therefore they should be equally compatible with both the “not many” and the “many not” contexts. Jacobs predicts that (4) should only have the inverse reading while in (5) only the linear one should be available. Results: I-Topicalizations like (4) containing “viele” and “nicht” were rated as good in contexts that only allow the inverse reading. Contrary to Büring’s predictions they were rated as bad in contexts that only permit the linear reading. Focus constructions like (5) showed exactly the opposite pattern. (This effect showed up in a highly significant interaction: F1(1,9)=72,979; p<0,01; F2(1,4)=528,757; p<0,01) Tag der Linguistik Judgements based on intuitions – a good basis for semantic theory? Project Leader: Jürgen Pafel Oliver Bott, Janina Radó, Andreas Konietzko, Katrin Petodnig, Tanja Werner Projekt A4 – Semantik und Intuition SFB 441 – Linguistische Datenstrukturen Universität Tübingen Further questions To answer the question whether there is a scope difference between (2) and (3) different quantifiers like “viele” versus “zwei Drittel” will have to be tested. Further interesting questions are: Do grammatical factors also play a role, beside intonation, in determining scope in quantifier/negation-constructions? Some candidates are subjecthood and position of the constituent bearing the rise accent. Is there a principled difference between the role of intonation in quantifier/negation- versus quantifier/quantifier-sentences? Is there a special meaning associated with the I-Topic contour cf. (1a)? References Bard E., Robertson D. & Sorace A. (1996). Magnitude Estimation of Linguistic Acceptability. Language 72 (1), Büring D. (1997). The Great Scope Inversion Conspiracy. Linguistics and Philosophy 20, S. 175 – 194 Jacobs, J. (1997). I-Topikalisierung. Linguistische Berichte 168, S © Institut für Linguistik, 9. Dezember 2003 I-Topicalizations: What do they mean? The prototypical Alle/Nicht-cases are the clear instances of I-Topicalization. (1)a) /ALLE Politiker sind NICHT\ korrupt. (not all politicians are corrupt.) For (1a) only the inverse reading is possible as explained by Büring, 1997; Jacobs, 1997 and others. However, once we consider more complicated sentences, contradictory judgements have been reported: (2)Zwei /DRITTEL der Politiker sind NICHT\ korrupt. (Büring, 1997; p. 188 f.) (3)/VIELEN Lesern werden die Verrisse NICHT\ einleuchten. (Jacobs, 1997; p. 110) For (2) Büring claims that both readings are possible, whereas Jacobs states that in (3) only the inverse reading is available. Some questions are: Are (2) and (3) really the “same”? How can we get clear reliable data about sentences like (2) and (3)? (4)/VIELE Legeplättchen hat Kind A NICHT\ in seiner Spielecke. (I-Topic) (5)VIELE\ Legeplättchen hat Kind A nicht in seiner Spielecke. (Focus) combined with disambiguiting contexts: ¬many: (inverse) many¬: (linear) child A: