Symbolically-cued asymmetric reaches result in spatial interference during initiation and execution Jarrod Blinch and Romeo Chua School of Human Kinetics.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Results and Discussion Logan Pedersen & Dr. Mei-Ching Lien School of Psychological Science, College of Liberal Arts Introduction A classic finding in Psychology.
Advertisements

DOES THE LINEAR SYNERGY HYPOTHESIS GENERALIZE BEYOUND THE SHOULDER AND ELBOW IN MULTI-JOINT REACHING MOVEMENTS? James S. Thomas*, Daniel M Corcos†,, and.
Does radical type frequency reliably affect character recognition? Zih-Nian, Cong & Jei-Tun, Wu Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei,
Science of SpinScience of Spin Tony Purnell 2011 GSEF.
Fig. 1. Reward-based, free-choice task and monkey's performance. (A) Time chart of events that occurred during the task. (B) Diagram of large-reward probabilities.
Examining the Signal Examine the signal using a very high-speed system, for example, a 50 MHz digital oscilloscope.
THE EFFECTS OF AGING ON DISCRETE AND CONTINUOUS MOTOR COORDINATION A. S. Bangert 1, C. M. Walsh 2,3, A. E. Boonin 1,4, E. Anderson 4, D. J. Goble 4, P.
AGE AND BIMANUAL MOTOR COORDINATION: IMPLICATIONS FOR CALLOSAL DECLINES Ashley S. Bangert 1, Christine M. Walsh 2, Anna E. Boonin 1, 3, Emily Anderson.
A kinematic cost Reza Shadmehr. Subject’s performanceMinimum jerk motion Flash and Hogan, J Neurosci 1985 Point to point movements generally exhibit similar.
COORDINATION DYNAMICS AND ATTENTIONAL COSTS OF CONTINUOUS AND DISCONTINUOUS BIMANUAL CIRCLE DRAWING MOVEMENTS Jeff Summers, Sabrina Maeder, Cynthia Hiraga.
Interference during the preparation of bimanual movements: the role of asymmetric starting locations, movement amplitudes, and target locations Jarrod.
Hand trajectories reveal hidden cognitive states Ken Nakayama a Joo-Hyun Song b Matthew Finkbeiner c Alfonso Caramazza a a Harvard University b Smith-Kettlewell.
Methods Inhibition of Return was used as a marker of attention capture.  After attention goes to a location it is inhibited from returning later. Results.
The effect of varying the visual context on trajectory planning and perceptual awareness of one’s own performance 1 Psychology and Neurocognition Laboratory.
KINEMATICS/KINETICS CORRELATIONS OF ARM MOTOR CONTROL DURING CORIOLIS PERTURBATIONS. A. Pierobon, S.B. Bortolami, J.R. Lackner*, P. DiZio. Ashton Graybiel.
THE EFFECTS OF TRAINING ON SPINE-HIP RATIO IN DANCERS DURING A REACHING TASK Erica L. Dickinson, and James S. Thomas School of Physical Therapy, Ohio University,
Acknowledgement Work supported by NINDS (grant NS39845), NIMH (grants MH42900 and 19116) and the Human Frontier Science Program Methods Fullhead.
Disrupting face biases in visual attention Anna S. Law, Liverpool John Moores University Stephen R. H. Langton, University of Stirling Introduction Method.
This article and any supplementary material should be cited as follows: Brokaw EB, Murray T, Nef T, Lum PS. Retraining of interjoint arm coordination after.
The role of visuo-spatial working memory in attention to eye gaze Anna S. Law, Liverpool John Moores University Stephen R. H. Langton, University of Stirling.
Ten participants made bimanual reaching movements with a movement time goal of 500 ms. The trajectories of the hands were recorded with an Optotrak with.
It is believed that when inter-trial time is not controlled, the CI effect will occur (i.e., random practice will outperform blocked practice in retention),
Spatiotemporal coupling of bimanual reach-to-grasp movements
Audio-spinal reflex response in human limb muscles
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(10):16. doi: / Figure Legend:
The involvement of visual and verbal representations in a quantitative and a qualitative visual change detection task. Laura Jenkins, and Dr Colin Hamilton.
2 Jarrod Blinch1, Youngdeok Kim1, Romeo Chua2 1
Within a Mixed-Frequency Visual Environment
During Anterior-Posterior Optic Flow
Information processing of bimanual reaching movements
An Interference Effect of Observed Biological Movement on Action
Heather L. Dean, Maureen A. Hagan, Bijan Pesaran  Neuron 
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Decision Making during the Psychological Refractory Period
Choosing Goals, Not Rules: Deciding among Rule-Based Action Plans
An Interference Effect of Observed Biological Movement on Action
Differential Modulation of Cardiac Ca2+ Channel Gating by β-Subunits
Representation of Object Weight in Human Ventral Visual Cortex
Bimanual reaches with symbolic cues exhibit errors in target selection
Heather L. Dean, Maureen A. Hagan, Bijan Pesaran  Neuron 
Goal-Related Activity in V4 during Free Viewing Visual Search
Sensory Deprivation Unmasks a PKA-Dependent Synaptic Plasticity Mechanism that Operates in Parallel with CaMKII  Neil Hardingham, Nick Wright, James Dachtler,
Alteration of Visual Perception prior to Microsaccades
Volume 81, Issue 6, Pages (March 2014)
Trajectory Encoding in the Hippocampus and Entorhinal Cortex
Feature- and Order-Based Timing Representations in the Frontal Cortex
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Responses of Collicular Fixation Neurons to Gaze Shift Perturbations in Head- Unrestrained Monkey Reveal Gaze Feedback Control  Woo Young Choi, Daniel.
Medial Prefrontal and Subcortical Mechanisms Underlying the Acquisition of Motor and Cognitive Action Sequences in Humans  Etienne Koechlin, Adrian Danek,
Sensory Deprivation Unmasks a PKA-Dependent Synaptic Plasticity Mechanism that Operates in Parallel with CaMKII  Neil Hardingham, Nick Wright, James Dachtler,
A Dedicated Binding Mechanism for the Visual Control of Movement
Neural Mechanisms of Visual Motion Perception in Primates
Vivek R. Athalye, Karunesh Ganguly, Rui M. Costa, Jose M. Carmena 
Brian D Corneil, Etienne Olivier, Douglas P Munoz  Neuron 
Uma R. Karmarkar, Dean V. Buonomano  Neuron 
Ethan S. Bromberg-Martin, Masayuki Matsumoto, Okihide Hikosaka  Neuron 
Volume 95, Issue 5, Pages e5 (August 2017)
Kevin G. Guise, Matthew L. Shapiro  Neuron 
Ca-Activation and Stretch-Activation in Insect Flight Muscle
CHAPTER – 1.2 UNCERTAINTIES IN MEASUREMENTS.
Normal Movement Selectivity in Autism
Volume 76, Issue 4, Pages (November 2012)
Sébastien Marti, Jean-Rémi King, Stanislas Dehaene  Neuron 
Prefrontal Neurons Coding Suppression of Specific Saccades
Motor Planning, Not Execution, Separates Motor Memories
Gregor Rainer, Earl K Miller  Neuron 
Gijsbert Stoet, Lawrence H Snyder  Neuron 
Volume 65, Issue 4, Pages (February 2010)
Nonvisual Motor Training Influences Biological Motion Perception
Responsibility Assignment in Redundant Systems
Presentation transcript:

Symbolically-cued asymmetric reaches result in spatial interference during initiation and execution Jarrod Blinch and Romeo Chua School of Human Kinetics at the University of British Columbia When both arms attempt to reach at the same time with different paths, interference occurs that forces the paths to become more similar than desired (Kelso, Putnam, & Goodman, 1983; Swinnen, Walter, & Shapiro, 1988). This spatial interference occurs during movement execution, but spatial interference can also occur during premovement planning. For example, imagine a reaction time task where each arm is cued to make either a long or short movement at the same time. When these movements are cued with symbols L or S to indicate a long or short movement (Figure 1, right), the reaction times are longer for movements with unequal amplitudes (asymmetric: L S or S L) than movements with equal amplitudes (symmetric: L L or S S). This increased reaction time for asymmetric movements is the result of interference during premovement planning, specifically the translation of two different symbolic cues (L S or S L) to movement amplitudes. Interestingly, the reaction times for asymmetric and symmetric reaches are equal when directly cueing the movements by illuminating the targets (Figure 1, left) (Diedrichsen, Hazeltine, Kennerley, & Ivry, 2001). Directly-cued movements avoid interference during initiation as they require less translation than movements cued symbolically (Hazeltine, Diedrichsen, Kennerley, & Ivry, 2003). Spatial interference during movement execution has yet to be examined in this task, but it could result in amplitude assimilation of asymmetric reaches (the short movement would become longer and vice versa). Will there be greater amplitude assimilation for symbolically- cued asymmetric reaches than directly-cued ones? Introduction Conclusions Symbolically-cued asymmetric reaches, as compared to directly-cued asymmetric reaches, have more processing demands on response selection for the cue-to-target translations (Hazeltine et al., 2003). This has been shown to result in interference during premovement planning (Diedrichsen et al., 2001). We also found that it results in greater amplitude assimilation, which is corrected by the end of the movement. Amplitude assimilation is a form of spatial interference during movement execution. Therefore, symbolically-cued asymmetric reaches result in spatial interference during initiation and execution. References Diedrichsen, J, Hazeltine, E, Kennerley, S, & Ivry, RB (2001). Moving to directly cued locations abolishes spatial interference during bimanual actions. Psychological Science, 12(6), Hazeltine, E, Diedrichsen, J, Kennerley, S, & Ivry, RB (2003). Bimanual cross-talk during reaching movements is primarily related to response selection, not the specification of motor parameters. Psychological Research, 67(1), Kelso, JA, Putnam, CA, & Goodman, D (1983). On the space-time structure of human interlimb co- ordination. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35(2), Swinnen, SP, Walter, CB, & Shapiro, DC (1988). The coordination of limb movements with different kinematic patterns. Brain and Cognition, 8(3), Eighteen participants made bimanual reaching movements in six blocks with either direct (Figure 1, left) or symbolic cues (Figure 1, right). Movement trajectories were recorded with an Optotrak at a sample frequency of 500 Hz. Methods Figure 1 Results The reaction times of symbolically-cued movements were slower than directly- cued movements (Figure 2). Asymmetric reaches were initiated slower than symmetric reaches. A significant cue by reach interaction replicated Diedrichsen and colleagues (2001) and showed that symbolic asymmetric reaches were initiated 56.7 ms slower than symbolic symmetric reaches. Reaction times of direct asymmetric reaches were also slower than direct symmetric reaches, but only by 7.8 ms. The mean time-normalized forward positions are shown for a representative participant in Figure 3. Directly-cued movements are on the left and symbolically- cued movements are on the right; symmetric reaches are shown in the top row and asymmetric reaches are shown in the bottom row (overlaid on the same symmetric reaches as the top row). Solid lines are mean forward positions for the left arm, and the dotted lines are one standard deviation from the participant’s mean. Comparing the overlaid asymmetric reaches to the symmetric reaches (Figure 3, bottom), gives an indication of amplitude assimilation. Visual inspection revealed that there was greater amplitude assimilation with symbolic cues (Figure 3, bottom-right) than direct cues (Figure 3, bottom-left). Subtracting the symmetric reaches from the asymmetric reaches normalized the forward positions of asymmetric reaches. The potential amplitude assimilation was analyzed by calculating the root mean squared error of the normalized positions. Symbolically-cued movements had greater assimilation than directly-cued movements (Figure 4). Short movements also had greater assimilation than long movements. There was also a significant cue by movement amplitude interaction that revealed that short movements had a greater increase in amplitude assimilation from direct to symbolic cues than long movements. Figure 4 Figure 3. Forward positions by percent time. Figure 2 L L L S S S S L