Overview Historically, invertebrates have not been granted with possessing higher cognitive faculties Instead, they have been thought to rely on reflexes,

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
F exam 2 F learning in a natural environment F special case... flower learning F odor learning in the proboscis extension reflex F summary PART 4: BEHAVIORAL.
Advertisements

Getting the most out of insect-related data. A major issue for pollinator studies is to find out what affects the number of various insects. Example from.
READ CHAPTER 4.1 UP TO AND INCLUDING PAGE 67. READ CHAPTER 4.1 UP TO AND INCLUDING PAGE 67. WHAT DO WE MEAN BY SAYING THAT ACCORDING TO ARISTOTLE, THERE.
EPM: Chs V & VI Pete Mandik Chairman, Department of Philosophy Coordinator, Cognitive Science Laboratory William Paterson University, New Jersey USA “RED!”
Children’s Thinking Lecture 3 Methodological Preliminaries Introduction to Piaget.
Visual Hemifields and Perceptual Grouping Sarah Theobald & Nestor Matthews Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville OH USA The human.
Searching for the NCC We can measure all sorts of neural correlates of these processes…so we can see the neural correlates of consciousness right? So what’s.
I. Face Perception II. Visual Imagery. Is Face Recognition Special? Arguments have been made for both functional and neuroanatomical specialization for.
Organizational Notes no study guide no review session not sufficient to just read book and glance at lecture material midterm/final is considered hard.
Introduction to Artificial Intelligence Ruth Bergman Fall 2004.
Pattern Classification All materials in these slides were taken from Pattern Classification (2nd ed) by R. O. Duda, P. E. Hart and D. G. Stork, John.
Pattern Recognition Pattern - complex composition of sensory stimuli that the human observer may recognize as being a member of a class of objects Issue.
Learning Complex Concepts II Attributes The following notation system is used to represent the structure of concepts. Notation Rules These are represented.
Visual Imagery One of the greatest problems confronting psychology is the nature of mental representation. Part of this debate is the nature of representations.
Concept Learning Concepts are categories of stimuli that have certain features in common. The shapes on the left are all members of a conceptual category:
Knowledge Representation: Images and Propositions Chapter 7.
What is Cognitive Science? … is the interdisciplinary study of mind and intelligence, embracing philosophy, psychology, artificial intelligence, neuroscience,
Towards a true neural stance on consciousness by Victor A.F. Lamme (2006) Group 10: Chi-Hang Lau, Anita Leung, Clarisse Miguel, Elisa Tsan, Alistair Wong.
What is Cognitive Science? … is the interdisciplinary study of mind and intelligence, embracing philosophy, psychology, artificial intelligence, neuroscience,
December 2, 2014Computer Vision Lecture 21: Image Understanding 1 Today’s topic is.. Image Understanding.
Conceptual modelling. Overview - what is the aim of the article? ”We build conceptual models in our heads to solve problems in our everyday life”… ”By.
Discrimination-Shift Problems Background This type of task has been used to compare concept learning across species as well as across a broad range of.
Iris Balodis Scientific Teaching Fellows Course Teachable Tidbit: Face Perception.
Theories of Development. Cognitive Development Early psychologists believed that children were not capable of meaningful thought and that there actions.
© Curriculum Foundation1 Section 2 The nature of the assessment task Section 2 The nature of the assessment task There are three key questions: What are.
1 of 27 PSYC 4310/6310 Advanced Experimental Methods and Statistics © 2013, Michael Kalsher Michael J. Kalsher Department of Cognitive Science Adv. Experimental.
Why Children Draw  To communicate their own feelings, ideas and experiences and express them in ways that someone else can understand.  Provides a nonverbal.
November 25, 2014Computer Vision Lecture 20: Object Recognition IV 1 Creating Data Representations The problem with some data representations is that the.
Chapter One Theories of Learning
Chapter 1: Introduction – Historical Developments in the study of the Mind Learning theory can trace its roots to the philosophy of Rene Descartes (1596-
Lecture 1 Note: Some slides and/or pictures are adapted from Lecture slides / Books of Dr Zafar Alvi. Text Book - Aritificial Intelligence Illuminated.
MODULE 23 COGNITION/THINKING. THINKING Thinking is a cognitive process in which the brain uses information from the senses, emotions, and memory to create.
Boris Babenko Department of Computer Science and Engineering University of California, San Diego Semi-supervised and Unsupervised Feature Scaling.
Copyright © by Holt, Rinehart and Winston. All rights reserved. ResourcesChapter menu To View the presentation as a slideshow with effects select “View”
Theories of First Language Acquisition
September 23, 2014Computer Vision Lecture 5: Binary Image Processing 1 Binary Images Binary images are grayscale images with only two possible levels of.
Artificial Intelligence Bodies of animals are nothing more than complex machines - Rene Descartes.
LOGIC AND ONTOLOGY Both logic and ontology are important areas of philosophy covering large, diverse, and active research projects. These two areas overlap.
Chapter 10 Cognition, Language, Creativity. Concepts Allow us to think abstractly Concept formation: classify information into meaningful categories (belonging.
Honeybee learning and memory. Honeybee brain AL Moth AL.
Philosophy 224 Responding to the Challenge. Taylor, “The Concept of a Person” Taylor begins by noting something that is going to become thematic for us.
Paradoxical False Memory for Objects After Brain Damage Stephanie M. McTighe 1,2 ; Rosemary A. Cowell 3, Boyer D. Winters 4, Timothy J. Bussey 1,2 and.
Thinking part I Mental Representations and Visual Imagery Mind Reading
Concepts in the Light of Evolution Session 3 Reza Maleeh Institute of Cognitive Science University of Osnabrück University of Osnabrueck1.
348: Representation - Propositions. Form a mental image of this picture Which of the pictures on the next slide are part of this picture?
Course Overview  What is AI?  What are the Major Challenges?  What are the Main Techniques?  Where are we failing, and why?  Step back and look at.
MAX This is MAX. He is a brain in a vat. (and this is a new take on an old thought experiment) Unlike other envatted brains however, the Physical Reality.
Sixteen Questions About Software Reuse William B. Frakes and Christopher J. Fox Communications of the ACM.
Chapter 6 - Standardized Measurement and Assessment
Swarms MONT 104Q – Mathematical Journeys, November 2015.
PSY 432: Personality Chapter 1: What is Personality?
Irfan Ullah Department of Information and Communication Engineering Myongji university, Yongin, South Korea Copyright © solarlits.com.
Eye Movements and Working Memory Marc Pomplun Department of Computer Science University of Massachusetts at Boston Homepage:
Artificial Intelligence Hossaini Winter Outline book : Artificial intelligence a modern Approach by Stuart Russell, Peter Norvig. A Practical Guide.
Decision Support Systems INF421 & IS341. Course Textbook Title: Decision Behavior, Analysis and Support Authors: Simon French John Maule Nadia Papamichail.
DATA VISUALIZATION BOB MARSHALL, MD MPH MISM FAAFP FACULTY, DOD CLINICAL INFORMATICS FELLOWSHIP.
 Good for:  Knowledge level content  Evaluating student understanding of popular misconceptions  Concepts with two logical responses.
Artificial Intelligence
Copyright © 2010, 2007, 2004 Pearson Education, Inc. Chapter 21 More About Tests and Intervals.
What is cognitive psychology?
Mental Rotation of Naturalistic Human Faces
Artificial Intelligence (CS 370D)
Done Done Course Overview What is AI? What are the Major Challenges?
Computer Vision Lecture 5: Binary Image Processing
Course Instructor: knza ch
Creating Data Representations
Mark G. Baxter Involvement of Medial Temporal Lobe Structures in Memory and Perception.
Robot Intelligence Kevin Warwick.
Reciprocity and Cooperation
Cognitive Level of Analysis: Cognitive Processes
Presentation transcript:

Overview Historically, invertebrates have not been granted with possessing higher cognitive faculties Instead, they have been thought to rely on reflexes, instincts, or other “hardwired” tendencies However, recent research on honeybees (Apis mellifera) has shown them to be capable of learning basic concepts, categorizing visual stimuli according to several criteria, and even discriminating between human faces These findings have broad implications for artificial life, neuroscience and cognitive science

Why Bees? Bees are highly social insects with complex social organizations and communication systems, including a dance language that is “second only to human language in its ability to communicate information” (Gould 2002) They are central-place foragers, which means they have to return to their hives after foraging, and they often navigate over long distances of up to several kilometers in doing so (Benard et al. 2006) They are flower-constant—that is, they feed off of flowers of one species—requiring them to discriminate between different flower species but also to generalize between slightly different flowers of the same species (ibid.)

Categorization in Honeybees Honeybees have shown an ability to categorize visual stimuli based on angle of orientation (van Hateran et al., 1990), bilateral symmetry (Giurfa et al., 1996), and radial symmetry (Horridge & Zhang, 1995) More recently, Stach et al. (2004) showed that honeybees can distinguish between sets of complex patterns that could not be categorized according to just one parameter (e.g., angle of orientation, symmetry, etc.)

Bilateral Symmetry Bees were first trained on sets of three stimuli, one symmetric and two asymmetric (or vice versa) During training, bees were rewarded for choosing the symmetric pattern and not rewarded for choosing either of the two asymmetric ones (or vice versa, with one asymmetric pattern and two symmetric ones) They were then tested using novel stimuli The bees performed at chance levels over the first several test trials, but after approximately the seventh trial, they showed an ability to understand the task and to distinguish between symmetric and asymmetric patterns

Bilateral Symmetry a)Training Stimuli b)Test stimuli c)Acquisition curve

Bilateral Symmetry Bees that were rewarded for choosing symmetric patterns hovered longer in front of the symmetric patterns than the bees rewarded for asymmetric patterns did in front of the asymmetric patterns This evidence suggests a predisposition (learned or otherwise) for symmetry in bees, which makes sense biologically However, even if this predisposition were assumed to be strictly “hardwired,” Gould (2002) notes that “we are still left to wonder what sort of mental leap allowed these bees to understand that this particular concept was the one that the experimenters wanted them to key in on” The presence of a “learning curve”—with an abrupt increase after seven trials—suggests that some sort of “mental leap” did in fact take place with the bees

Categorizing Multiple-Feature Sets More recently, bees have been shown to be able to generalize from sets of complex patterns and then to distinguish between novel stimuli belonging to these sets The bees’ success at these tasks suggest they are able to “build generic pattern representations” and to remember these patterns’ orientations “simultaneously in their appropriate positions” (Benard et al., 2006) This ability is impressive because these patterns cannot be categorized according to just one criterion

Categorizing Multiple-Feature Sets a)Training stimuli b)Test stimuli c)Acquisition curve

Human Facial Discrimination Dyer et. al (2005) trained bees to “visit target facial stimuli and to avoid similar distractor stimuli from a standard face recognition test used in human psychology” As with the earlier tests, bees were rewarded for correct “responses” during training with a sucrose solution, but unlike with other tests, they were also punished for incorrect responses using a quinine solution The bees that did show evidence of learning during the training period (five of the original seven) were able to distinguish target faces from distractors at a rate of 80% or better However, the bees—like humans—performed much worse when both the target and distractor faces were turned upside- down

Human Facial Discrimination a)Foraging setup for bees b)“Hmmm…” (A bee evaluates one of its choices.) c)Target stimuli (top row) vs. distractor stimuli (bottom row); percent correct for each target/distractor column

Human Facial Discrimination The authors note that previous studies had shown bees to be capable of recognizing and distinguishing flower types—and even the faces of their conspecifics—but that “to our knowledge this current study is the first report that invertebrates have sufficient neural flexibility to learn how to discriminate between and recognise faces of other species.” Given the extensive research on facial recognition in humans, these finding are interesting because they suggest that “face recognition is a task that can be solved, at least to a certain level, by a general neural system that has a reasonable degree of plasticity“—and one that is several orders of magnitude smaller (in terms of the number of neurons involved) than that of humans

So What? “This is all very well,” one might say, “but learning to recognize patterns or faces is a fairly trivial performance, especially if it is done the hard way, by reward and punishment. It would be a different matter if these creatures could form their own concepts in quiet meditation, without an external tutor telling them what is important. But they never will, because abstraction is one of the powers that is unique to the human mind.”

So What? “But look,” says another philosopher, “I just watched an abstraction being made by one of these creatures. Or if you wish, we can say that a generalization has taken place from particular patterns indicating bilateral symmetry (or whatever else) to the general concept ‘bilateral symmetry.’” And so on…

Double Standards? The previous dialogue (“:adapted” from pp of Braitenberg’s Vehicles) warns against adopting double standards, whether in terms of machines or lower animals such as bees On this note, Gould (2002) has suggested three possibilities regarding cognition in lower animals versus cognition in humans and non-human primates (or mammals, or vertebrates): 1.Cognition has evolved as needed among animals, independent of size, number of legs, etc. Cognitive differences between species are quantitative, not qualitative. 2.Behaviors that require cognition in humans, primates, rodents, etc., are “innate” or “hardwired” in “lower” animals such as bees. Thus it could be that [such behaviors in bees are] hardwired; in rodents and primates, on the other hand, the ability is genuinely cognitive--that is, it is not a consequence of innate preparation.” 3.Certain human capacities—such as categorization, abstraction, etc.— that we commonly label as “cognitive” are perhaps not so “cognitive” after all. Or perhaps the definition of “cognition” isn’t so clear after all.

Closing Points Given the available evidence, it seems wise to view intelligence (like life, or consciousness) as existing along a continuum rather than on a binary, “yes or no” scale The questions raised by research on animal cognition, much like Braitenberg’s Vehicles, parallel some of the main philosophical issues in cognitive science, e.g., “What are concepts?” “What is cognition?” In terms of artificial life and neuroscience, findings from research on honeybees could be seen as encouraging. They have shown that certain complex behaviors can be achieved by nervous systems that are much smaller and “simpler” than those of humans, primates, or other vertebrates. Thus, these organisms can serve as valuable models for neuroscience, and their behaviors make for worthy (if not exactly easy) goals for artificial life to try to emulate

References Benard, Julie, Silke Stach and Martin Giurfa “Categorization of Visual Stimuli in the Honeybee Apis mellifera.” Animal Cognition 9 (4): Braitenberg, Valentino. Vehicles: Experiments in Synthetic Psychology Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Dyer, A. G., Neumeyer, C. and Chittka, L “Honeybee (Apis mellifera) Vision Can Discriminate Between and Recognise Images of Human Faces.” J. Exp. Biol. 208: Giurfa, M., B. Eichmann and R. Menzel “Symmetry Perception in an Insect.” Nature 382: 458–461. Gould, J. L “Can Honey Bees Create Cognitive Maps?” In M. Bekoff and C. Allen (eds.), The Cognitive Animal: Empirical and Theoretical Perspectives on Animal Cognition. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Hateren, J.H., M.V. Srinivasan, and P.B. Wait “Pattern Recognition in Bees: Orientation Discrimination.” J. Comp. Physiol. A 197: Horridge, G.A. and S.W. Zhang “Pattern Vision in Honeybees (Apis mellifera): Flower-like Patterns with No Predominant Orientation.” J. Insect Physiol. 41: 681–688 Background images taken from