Randomization: A Missing Component of the Single-Case Research Methodological Standards Adapted from Kratochwill, T. R., & Levin, J. R. (2010). Enhancing.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Experimental and Ex Post Facto Designs
Advertisements

Chapter 9 Overview of Alternating Treatments Designs.
Chapter 15 Evaluation Recognizing Success. Social Work Evaluation and Research Historically –Paramount to the work of early social work pioneers Currently.
Experimental Research Designs
+ Evidence Based Practice University of Utah Presented by Will Backner December 2009 Training School Psychologists to be Experts in Evidence Based Practices.
PTP 560 Research Methods Week 4 Thomas Ruediger, PT.
Single -Subject Designs - Ch 5 “Data collection allows teachers to make statements about the direction and magnitude of behavioral changes” (p. 116). In.
Quasi-Experimental Designs
Chapter 12 Single-Case Research Designs ♣ ♣ Introduction   Single-Case Designs   Methodological Considerations in Using Single- Case Designs   Criteria.
Educational Research by John W. Creswell. Copyright © 2002 by Pearson Education. All rights reserved. Slide 1 Chapter 11 Experimental and Quasi-experimental.
Single-Subject Research
L1 Chapter 11 Experimental and Quasi- experimental Designs Dr. Bill Bauer.
Experimental Research
Single-Subject Designs
Experiments and Observational Studies.  A study at a high school in California compared academic performance of music students with that of non-music.
Non-true-experimental Designs, cont. PSY 231 Research Methods in Psychology.
2.4. Design in quantitative research Karl Popper’s notion of falsification and science – If a theory is testable and incompatible with possible empirical.
Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved. Doing Research in Behavior Modification Chapter 22.
Doing Research in Behavior Modification
Overview of MSP Evaluation Rubric Gary Silverstein, Westat MSP Regional Conference San Francisco, February 13-15, 2008.
بسم الله الرحمن الرحيم * this presentation about :- “experimental design “ * Induced to :- Dr Aidah Abu Elsoud Alkaissi * Prepared by :- 1)-Hamsa karof.
Chapter 11 Research Methods in Behavior Modification.
Single-Case Research: Standards for Design and Analysis Thomas R. Kratochwill University of Wisconsin-Madison.
Principles of Scientifically Credible Intervention Research Brief Review and Transition to Single-Case Intervention Research Joel R. Levin University of.
Power Point Slides by Ronald J. Shope in collaboration with John W. Creswell Chapter 11 Experimental Designs.
Statistical Randomization Techniques for Single-Case Intervention Data Statistical Randomization Techniques for Single-Case Intervention Data Joel R. Levin.
Moving from Single-Case to Conventional Group Intervention Designs, and Vice Versa Joel R. Levin University of Arizona.
INTERNATIONAL SOCIETY FOR TECHNOLOGY IN EDUCATION working together to improve education with technology Using Evidence for Educational Technology Success.
Classroom-Based Applications of Single-Case Designs: Methodological and Statistical Issues Joel R. Levin University of Arizona.
Current Methodological Issues in Single Case Research David Rindskopf, City University of New York Rob Horner, University of Oregon.
Educational Research: Competencies for Analysis and Application, 9 th edition. Gay, Mills, & Airasian © 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved.
Single-Subject Experimental Research
Experimental Design ã Dependent variable (DV): Variable observed to determine the effects of an experimental manipulation (behavior) ã Independent variable.
Evaluating Impacts of MSP Grants Hilary Rhodes, PhD Ellen Bobronnikov February 22, 2010 Common Issues and Recommendations.
Random Thoughts On Enhancing the Scientific Credibility of Single-Case Intervention Research: Randomization to the Rescue Thomas R. Kratochwill and Joel.
Educational Research: Competencies for Analysis and Application, 9 th edition. Gay, Mills, & Airasian © 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved.
SOCW 671 # 8 Single Subject/System Designs Intro to Sampling.
Evaluating Impacts of MSP Grants Ellen Bobronnikov Hilary Rhodes January 11, 2010 Common Issues and Recommendations.
Experimental Research Methods in Language Learning Chapter 4 Experimental Research Designs.
Randomized Single-Case Intervention Designs Joel R
General Overview and History of Statistical Analysis of Single-Case Intervention Data Joel R. Levin University of Arizona.
Statistical Randomization Techniques for Single-Case Intervention Data Statistical Randomization Techniques for Single-Case Intervention Data Joel R. Levin.
Single-Subject and Correlational Research Bring Schraw et al.
Experimental and Ex Post Facto Designs
Brief Comments on Single-Case Effect Sizes and Meta-Analyses* Joel R. Levin University of Arizona *Based, in part, on Kratochwill, T. R., & Levin, J. R.
Single- Subject Research Designs
Statistical Randomization Tests: Issues and Applications Randomization Tests versus Permutation Tests Randomization Tests versus Permutation Tests Test.
IES Project Director’s Meeting June 2010 Rob Horner University of Oregon.
Research Design Quantitative. Quantitative Research Design Quantitative Research is the cornerstone of evidence-based practice It provides the knowledge.
IES Single-Case Research Institute: Training Visual Analysis Rob Horner University of Oregon
1 Negative Results and Publication Bias in Single- Case Research Applications of the WWC Standards in Literature Reviews.
Chapter 6 Selecting a Design. Research Design The overall approach to the study that details all the major components describing how the research will.
RESEARCH DESIGN Experimental Designs  
Chapter 11: Quasi-Experimental and Single Case Experimental Designs
Goals of the Presentation
Chapter 8 Experimental Design The nature of an experimental design
Joel R. Levin University of Arizona
Statistical Randomization Techniques for Single-Case Intervention Data and Introduction to ExPRT Statistical Software Joel R. Levin University of.
11 Single-Case Research Designs.
Research Methods in Behavior Change Programs
Masked Visual Analysis (MVA)
Research Methods: Concepts and Connections First Edition
DAY 2 Single-Case Design Drug Intervention Research Tom Kratochwill
Randomization: A Missing Component of the Single-Case Research Methodological Standards Joel R. Levin University of Arizona Adapted from Kratochwill, T.
RESEARCH METHODS Lecture 33
Research Design Quantitative.
Small-n Designs.
Single-Case Intervention Research Training Institute
DAY 2 Single-Case Design Drug Intervention Research Tom Kratochwill
Masked Visual Analysis (MVA)
Presentation transcript:

Randomization: A Missing Component of the Single-Case Research Methodological Standards Adapted from Kratochwill, T. R., & Levin, J. R. (2010). Enhancing the scientific credibility of single-case intervention research: Randomization to the rescue. Psychological Methods, 15,

Why Randomization? The single-case designs and analyses to be promoted here receive high marks with respect to two critical research validity criteria: Internal Validity (a research design issue) Elevates the status of single-case research by increasing the scientific credibility of its methodology Statistical-Conclusion Validity (a data-analysis issue) Legitimizes the conduct of various statistical tests and one’s interpretation of the results

Traditional Single-Case Designs Basic design (AB) and various extensions Reversal (or withdrawal or “operant”) design (ABAB) Alternating and simultaneous treatment designs; also ABABAB…AB (or AB k ) Multiple-probe design Changing criterion design Multiple-baseline design

Four Single-Case Design-and-Analysis Randomization Variations 1.Within-Case Intervention Randomization 2.Between-Case Intervention Randomization 3.Case Randomization 4.Intervention Start-Point Randomization

1. Within-Case Intervention Randomization With within-case (or phase) randomization, the order in which the A and B phases are administered is randomly determined for each case (e.g., participant, pair, small group, classroom).

Days/Weeks/Months/Sessions Traditional Basic Design (AB)

AB Design “[I]nstead of automatically administering the two phases in an AB order, one could randomly determine which phase should come first.” (Levin, Marascuilo, & Hubert, 1978) Problems with randomizing phases? Problems with not randomizing phases? Levin, J. R., Marascuilo, L. A., & Hubert, L. J. (1978). N = nonparametric randomization tests. In T. R. Kratochwill (Ed.), Single subject research: Strategies for evaluating change (pp ). New York: Academic Press.

Reversal Design (ABAB)

Randomized Phase Designs Traditional ABAB Design Addresses maturation and carryover effects to some extent “[H]owever, systematic assignment (A preceding B in each pair) is not the same as random assignment (either within each pair or within the entire experiment).” (Levin et al., 1978) – other problems include Hawthorne/novelty effects Edgington, E. S. (1992). Nonparametric tests for single-case experiments. In T. R. Kratochwill & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Single-case research design and analysis (pp ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Onghena, P. (1992). Randomization tests for extensions and variation of ABAB single-case experimental designs: A rejoinder. Behavioral Assessment, 14,

Randomized Phase Designs For ABAB…AB and alternating-treatment designs, there are two basic types of phase randomization: simple and blocked. With simple randomization, the only constraint is that there be equal numbers of A and B phases in the design. With blocked randomization, additional constraints are imposed to control for order effects.

Randomized ABAB... AB Design With One Case, Two Within-Case Conditions, and 10 Time Periods 5 A and 5 B)

Replicated Randomized ABAB... AB Design With Four Cases, Two Within-Case Conditions, and 10 Time Periods (5 A and 5 B)

One Potentially Palatable Solution for the True Baseline (A) Situation? Prior to initiating the formal AB intervention study, include one or more mandatory baseline (adaptation, warmup) observations (A'). The administration order of the subsequent A and B phases is then randomly determined. The A' phase is not considered to be part of the actual study’s design or analysis.

Randomized Alternating Treatment Design (ATD) With One Case, Two Within-Case Conditions, and 13 Time Periods (Seven Mornings and Six Afternoons)

Randomized Alternating Intervention Design With Three Units, Two Within-Series Conditions, and 13 Time Periods (Seven Mornings and Six Afternoons)

2. Between-Case Intervention Randomization In some “between-case” multiple-intervention single- case designs, Treatment X (a control or intervention condition) is administered to one or more cases and Treatment Y (an alternative intervention condition) is administered to other cases. With between-case intervention randomization, which cases receive Treatment X and which receive Treatment Y is randomly determined.

A Two-Intervention (Between Cases) Example

3. Case Randomization With case randomization, cases are randomly assigned to the different replication positions within the design. Multiple-baseline designs, with their systematically staggered intervention start points, are uniquely suited to this type of randomization.

Traditional Multiple-Baseline Design Across Participants

Wampold, B. E., & Worsham, N. L. (1986). Randomization tests for multiple-baseline designs. Behavioral Assessment, 8,

Time Out for an Introduction to “Intervention Start Points” Historically, the intervention start point [and subsequent transition points] has [have] been “response guided” – preferred by many traditional SCD researchers. The intervention start point [and subsequent transition points] is [are] designated on an a priori basis by the researcher – preferred by traditional methodologists. The intervention start point [and subsequent transition points] is [are] randomly selected from a set of potential points that are designated as “acceptable” by the researcher – preferred by “new-age” SCD methodologists.

4. Intervention Start-Point Randomization With intervention start-point randomization, the actual A-to-B transition (“intervention start point”) is randomly selected from a set of researcher- designated “acceptable” (or “potential”) start points. This type of randomization can be implemented in single-case designs where A and B are either baseline and intervention conditions or two different intervention conditions.

AB Design With One Case (“Unit”), Two Within- Series Intervention Conditions, 20 Time Periods, and 13 Potential Intervention Start Points

Replicated AB Design With Three Cases (“Units”), Two Within-Series Intervention Conditions, 20 Time Periods, and 13 Potential Intervention Points for Each Case Marascuilo, L. A., & Busk, P. L. (1988). Combining statistics for multiple-baseline AB and replicated ABAB designs across subjects. Behavioral Assessment, 10,

Multiple-Baseline Design With 4 Randomized Cases (“Units”), Two Within-Series Conditions, 15 Time Periods, 3, 3, 2, and 2 Potential Intervention Start Points for Cases 1, 3, 2, and 4, Respectively, and a Staggered Intervention Introduction of at Least One Time Period

“Fascinating” Issue to Contemplate It can be argued that under certain conditions the Marascuilo and Busk (1988) replicated AB design, with randomly determined intervention start points for each case, should be considered very nearly equivalent (in terms of its scientific credibility) to a multiple-baseline design with random assignment of cases to the different multiple-baseline positions.

“Fascinating” Issue to Contemplate (cont.) Even better, the original Marascuilo-Busk procedure can be adapted to fit directly into a multiple-baseline structure. “Restricted” versions of the original procedure have recently been examined by Levin, Ferron, and Gafurov (2015). Levin, J. R., Ferron, J. F., & Gafurov, B. S. (2015). Comparison of randomization-test procedures for single-case multiple-baseline designs. Unpublished manuscript. University of Arizona, Tucson.

Other Randomized Start-Point Possibilities and Combinations Random Assignment of Interventions (Between Cases), Intervention Orders (Within Cases), and/or Intervention Start Points –when multiple interventions are included in the study

Levin, Ferron, & Gafurov’s (2014) Single-Case Randomized Intervention Start-Point, Random-Order Crossover Design Week Student 1 A A A A A A A B B B B B B B B Student 2 B B B B B B A A A A A A A A A Student 3 B B B B B B B B B A A A A A A Student 4 A A A A A B B B B B B B B B B Note: There are two interventions, A and B. Half of the students are randomly selected to receive an AB order of intervention administration and half to receive a BA order. With 15 sessions and a minimum of 5 sessions required for each intervention, each student receives a crossover start point randomly selected between Week 6 and Week 10 inclusive. This is a nice design because it can separate time/sequence effects and intervention effects by controlling for intervention order, Levin, J. R., Ferron, J. M., & Gafurov, B. S. (2014). Improved randomization tests for a class of single- case intervention designs. Journal of Modern Applied Statistical Methods, 13(2), 2-52; retrievable from

Levin & Wampold’s (1999) Replicated Simultaneous Start-Point Model Time Period Pair 1 X A A A A A A A A A A A B* B B B B B B B B Pair 1 Y A A A A A A A A A A A B* B B B B B B B B Pair 2 X A A A A A A A A A B* B B B B B B B B B B Pair 2 Y A A A A A A A A A B* B B B B B B B B B B Note: X and Y are two different intervention conditions, each of which is randomly assigned to a pair member in each pair. Potential intervention start points are between Time Periods 5 and 17 inclusive. *Randomly selected intervention start point for each pair of units

In Addition… Two interesting adaptations of the preceding design: 1. One can incorporate a mixture of a randomized component (start-point randomization) and a nonrandomized component (pair member classifications) to address either individual or group interaction/moderation hypotheses. In that situation, the X and Y members of each pair would represent different variable classifications of interest (e.g., gender, developmental level, classroom achievement).

In Addition… 2. X and Y could also be two different outcome measures, associated either with a single intervention or with two different interventions. For example, X could be a reading performance measure and Y an arithmetic performance measure. a.With a single intervention, A = Baseline, B = Intervention (e.g., B = a reading intervention), one could test the hypothesis that the intervention has comparable effects on the two measures. b.With two different interventions, A = Intervention 1, B = Intervention 2 (e.g., A = a reading intervention and B = an arithmetic intervention), one could test the hypothesis that the two interventions have comparable effects on their respective outcome measures.

Take-Home Message (to be Returned to Later in this Institute) Through various randomization schemes, it is possible to design single-case intervention studies that possess the same or similar scientific credibility characteristics as those of conventional randomized group intervention studies ‒ “best possible design” philosophy as a recurring theme With the additional inclusion of a sufficient number of replication components, who’s to say that a superbly implemented randomized single-case intervention study is less “valued” than a superbly implemented conventional randomized group intervention study?