Introduction Children with disruptive behaviours are at risk for low academic achievement and developing life-course persistent antisocial behaviour (Viding.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Absence from Australian Schools John Ainley. School attendance critically important not only for the individual who suffers educational disadvantage and.
Advertisements

A quantitative trait locus not associated with cognitive ability in children: a failure to replicate Hill, L. et al.
HOW PUPILS COPE WITH BULLYING: A LONGITUDINAL STUDY OF SUCCESSFUL AND UNSUCCESSFUL OUTCOMES Lorenzo Talamelli 1, Peter K Smith 1, Helen Cowie 2, Paul Naylor.
Self-Perceived Health in Early Adulthood: An examination of distal, childhood effects John Cairney, PhD Centre for Addiction and Mental Health Centre for.
Freshmeat : Psychosocial and Health Behaviors of Adolescents Transitioning to College ALBEE THERESE S. ONGSUCO, B.S., LAURA ANDERSON, PH.D.
Method Participants 184 five-year-old (M age=5.63, SD=0.22) kindergarten students from 30 classrooms in central Illinois Teacher ratings The second edition.
1 The SEP Gradient, Race, or the SEP Gradient and Race: Understanding Disparities in Child Health and Functioning Lisa Dubay, PhD, ScM The Urban Institute.
Lecture 2 ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE: AN INTRODUCTION
Felicia Meyer & William Bukowski Concordia University, Montreal, Canada 4 th International Stigma Conference January 23 rd, 2009.
Immigrant Adolescents Cultural Orientation and its Relationship to Academic and Social Adjustment Melissa Kull New York University Many thanks to Sita.
The Grade 9 Cohort of Fall 2000: Post-secondary Pathways Preliminary Analysis Presentation to HEQCO - June 15, 2009 Dr. Robert S. Brown Organizational.
Experimental and Quasiexperimental Designs Chapter 10 Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Canada, a division of Reed Elsevier Canada, Ltd.
Chapter 9: The relationship between physical activity and anxiety and depression Can physical activity beat the blues and help with your nerves?
Founding Family CEO Pay Incentives and Investment Policy: Evidence from a Structural Model Mieszko Mazur IESEG School of Management Betty Wu University.
Negative Parenting and Late Adolescents’ Mental Health: The Protective Function of Relationships with Grandparents Kristen E. Rabe, Bethany S. Quinn, &
Stereotype threat is defined as the experience of anxiety when faced with a confirmable stereotype (Steele & Aronson, 1995). Often anxiety obstructs cognitive.
Psychologists ’ and social workers ’ perceptions of personality disorders in substance abusing clients. Morten Hesse and Birgitte Thylstrup.
Jillian M. Wickery & Laura D. Pittman
Double Disadvantage: The impact of childhood maltreatment and community violence exposure on adolescent mental health Charlotte Cecil Molecules of Happiness:
Parenting and Youth Conduct Problems and Delinquency: Reciprocal Effects and Moderation by Callous-Unemotional Traits Luna C. Muñoz University of Central.
Indian Institute of Technology Hyderabad Study on Personality and Loneliness among the students of IIT Hyderabad -Jayashankar ( ES12B1011) Under the guidance.
Chapter 9: Experimental Design
Shame and Guilt © POSbase 2007Contributor Shame and guilt are moral, self-conscious, negative emotions. Moral emotions are those that are linked to the.
Visit our websites: PhD Study: Evaluation of the Efficacy of the Incredible.
Anxiety and Depression in Chemical Dependency Recovery Groups Erin Brock Hanover College.
The Discrepancy-Depression Association: Gender and Grade Differences Erin N. Stevens, M. C. Lovejoy, & Laura D. Pittman Northern Illinois University Introduction:
Social and Personality Development in Adolescents.
WHAT DO WE KNOW ABOUT SOCIAL INCLUSION?. SOCIAL INCLUSION Social inclusion is a process which ensures that those at risk of poverty and social exclusion.
Resilience in Aboriginal Children and Adolescents in Out-of-Home Care: A Test of an Initial Explanatory Model Katharine M. Filbert School of Psychology.
Correlates of 2D:4D Ratios and Fluctuating Asymmetry Patrick J. Cooper The Pennsylvania State University, Altoona INTRODUCTION The 2D:4D ratio refers to.
Different Pathways To Offending and Violence: An Examination Of The Differences Among Youths With Varying Histories Of Contact With The Juvenile Justice.
Reducing the Occurrence of Pedestrian Fatalities in the Transit Industry Though Suicide Prevention and Safety Promotion Keaton Zucker, M.S., & Patrick.
Implication of Gender and Perception of Self- Competence on Educational Aspiration among Graduates in Taiwan Wan-Chen Hsu and Chia- Hsun Chiang Presenter.
Acknowledgments: Data for this study were collected as part of the CIHR Team: GO4KIDDS: Great Outcomes for Kids Impacted by Severe Developmental Disabilities.
The impact of attachment and depression symptoms on multiple risk behaviors in post-war adolescents in northern Uganda Okello J, Nakimuli-Mpungu E, Klasen.
Ryoichi J. P. Noguchi, M.S., Michael M. Knepp, M.S., & Thomas H. Ollendick, Ph.D. INTRODUCTION METHOD  Studies of attention and memory have consistently.
Tattoos, Piercings, and Academic Success Student Name Mentor Name, Department Tattoos, Piercings, and Academic Success Student Name Mentor Name, Department.
Emotional Intelligence and Religion in Early Adolescence Sasha Collings and Kayla Kelder Missouri Western State University Faculty Sponsor: Dr. Teddi Deka.
Copyright © 2011 McGraw-Hill Australia Pty Ltd PPTs t/a Abnormal Psychology: Leading Researcher Perspectives 2e by Rieger et al Edited by Elizabeth.
 Are false memories more likely to develop when people are motivated to believe in the false event?  Sharman and Calacouris (2010)
High Narcissism and Low Self-Esteem as Risk Factors for the Development of Conduct Problems and Aggression in Children Kristy K. Adler 1, Christopher T.
The Broader Context of Relational Aggression in Adolescent Romantic Relationships Megan M. Schad, David E. Szwedo, Amanda Hare, Jill Antonishak, Joseph.
South West Public Health Scientific Conference - 1st February 2012
Gender differences in symptom reporting: the influence of psychological traits. Laura Goodwin Dr Stephen Fairclough Liverpool John Moores University BACKGROUND.
Self-Discrepancies and Depression: Abstract Reasoning Skills as a Moderator Erin N. Stevens, Christine Keeports, Nicole J. Holmberg, M. C. Lovejoy, Laura.
MethodIntroductionResults Discussion Factors Affecting Psychosocial Functioning in Serious Mental Illness and Implications for Treatment Jason E. Vogler,
INTRODUCTION METHOD RESULTS CONCLUSIONS Correspondence to: at the 21 st Annual Meeting of the Association for Psychological Science,
Personal Control over Development: Effects on the Perception and Emotional Evaluation of Personal Development in Adulthood.
ONLINE USAGE OF THEORY OF MIND CONTINUES TO DEVELOP IN LATE ADOLESCENCE Iroise Dumontheil, Ian A. Apperly, and Sarah-Jayne Blakemore.
 1,001 adolescent boys (47%) and girls (53%)  Fairly diverse: 58% Caucasian; 23% African American,12% Hispanic, 2% Asian, 5% Other  Age Range:
DİLARA ALTAN.  Concerns about the context of human behavior is increased accordingly Developmental and Social psychologists have conducted research that.
The Role of Close Family Relationships in Predicting Multisystemic Therapy Outcome: An Investigation of Sex Differences ABSTRACT BACKGROUND: Multisystemic.
General and Feeding Specific Behavior Problems in a Community Sample of Children Amy J. Majewski, Kathryn S. Holman & W. Hobart Davies University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee.
Parental, Temperament, & Peer Influences on Disordered Eating Symptoms Kaija M. Muhich, Alyssa Collura, Jessica Hick and Jennifer J. Muehlenkamp Psychology.
Minnesota Twin Family Study. The Study  An ongoing population-based, investigation of same-sex twin children and their parents that examines the origination.
Theories of Intelligence and Depression Among American Indian Youth Research-in-Progress There are many factors that may correlate with depressive symptoms.
The Ballerina’s Self-Concept: Self-Aspect Importance, Social Comparison, and Instructor Feedback at an Intensive Summer Program Katarina Walker and Beth.
Romantic Partners Promotion of Autonomy and Relatedness in Adolescence as a Predictor of Young Adult Emotion Regulation. Elenda T. Hessel, Emily L. Loeb,
Predicting Brain Size Samantha Stanley Jasmine Dumas Christopher Lee.
Developmental change and stability of personality
ERFCON th International Conference of the Faculty of Education and Rehabilitation Sciences University of Zagreb 17 – 19 May 2017, Zagreb SOCIAL COMPETENCE.
Key research: Van Leeuwen et al
Length of Homelessness as a Predictor of Separation Anxiety and
DESCRIPTIVES AND CORRELATIONS
Introduction Results Hypotheses Discussion Method
MICAMH Conference Feb, 2018 Charlotte Johnston
Introduction Results Methods Conclusions
Psychopathic traits in young children: concurrent and longitudinal associations with behavior problems Silvija Ručević, Tijana Borovac, Sandra Vučković.
Prosocial Behaviors in Adolescence
Developmental change and stability of personality
Presentation transcript:

Introduction Children with disruptive behaviours are at risk for low academic achievement and developing life-course persistent antisocial behaviour (Viding et al., 2009). Although two children with conduct problems (CP) may manifest similar disruptive behaviours, these behaviours could be driven by different underlying vulnerabilities and thus require different interventions. Callous-unemotional (CU) traits are one of the best researched markers of different causal pathways to antisocial behaviour (Frick & Viding, 2009) and can be used to distinguish between children who are capable of pre-meditated antisocial behaviour (CP/CU+) and children whose antisocial behaviour is more impulsive and threat reactive (CP/CU-). Researchers have suggested that two developmental pathways to disruptive/antisocial behaviour may exist, one involving low levels of guilt and empathy and another involving heightened shame (Eisenberg, 2000; Frick & Viding, 2009). Guilt is believed to be critical for moral actions and the development of conscience, whereas shame has been shown to be positively correlated with antisocial behaviour. Thus, whereas guilt is known to promote moral and reparative behaviour, high levels of shame appear to act in an opposite way (e.g. Tangney, 1998). This study examined guilt and shame responding in a community sample of adolescents who were grouped according to their level of CP and CU. Two vignette based measures were used. We predicted that CP/CU+ group would show lower levels of guilt responding and that that CP/CU- group would show higher levels of shame responding as compared with ability matched comparison children. Materials and methods Adolescents aged years old were recruited from mainstream comprehensive secondary schools using opportunity sampling. Study groups were screened from this sample based on questionnaire and ability data (final N=66). All groups were matched for age, gender and IQ (FSIQ of Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence; Wechsler, 1999): CP groups scored in the top tertile of the sample for CP (indexed by a combination score on the Conduct Disorder and Oppositional Defiant Disorder Scales of the Adolescent Symptom Inventory, Gadow & Sprafkin, 1997). These participants were further divided into CU+ (N=20) and CU- (N=18) groups using a median split on Inventory of Callous Unemotional Traits (ICU; Frick, 2003). Comparison group (no CP and low CU scores) was also selected (N=28). Measures Test of Self-Conscious Affect (TOSCA-3; Tangney, Dearing, Wagner, & Gramzow, 2000) - A self-report measure designed to examine shame- and guilt-proneness. Participants rate the likelihood of responding in a particular way following each scenario. Questionnaire of Subjective Emotions (QSE; Burnett, 2006) - A self-report measure with eight short scenarios used to examine the intensity of guilt and shame felt following imagined transgressions. Acknowledgments I would like to thank Essi Viding for supervising this project and Carol Cuthell for her help in recruiting participants. Results The TOSCA data was first inspected for outliers. Five participants who had an extreme score of +/- 3 standard deviations from their group mean were removed from the analysis, leaving 19 participants in the CP/CU+ group, 16 participants in CP/CU- group, and 26 participants in the comparison group. Mixed model ANOVA was conducted on the TOSCA measure: There was a significant main effect of group (F(2, 58)=4.98, p<0.01). There was a significant main effect of emotion (F(3, 174)=83.32, p<0.001). There was a significant group x emotion interaction (F(6, 174)=7.33, p<0.001). Planned comparisons were also run using independent samples t-tests corrected for multiple comparisons (See Fig. 1): Guilt CP/CU+ group showed less guilt than comparison group (t(45)=-3.953, p<0.001; d=1.13). CP/CU- group did not significantly differ from the comparison group (p=.52). Shame For shame, CP/CU+ group did not significantly differ from the comparison group (p=.16). CP/CU- group showed more shame than the comparison group (t(41)=2.466, p<0.02; d=0.75). These results show that in the TOSCA task CP/CU+ individuals displayed less guilt than comparison children, but CP/CU- individuals did not significantly differ from the comparison group. For shame, CP/CU+ individuals did not significantly differ from the comparison group, but CP/CU- individuals showed more shame responding than the comparison group Conclusions This study suggests that different moral emotion processing profiles may explain why different subgroups of children with disruptive/antisocial behaviours can find it difficult to perform reparative actions and behave in a morally acceptable way. Disruptive behaviours performed by CP/CU+ individuals may be attributable to low guilt, making it easier for them to continue aggressing against other people. Lack of guilt may also prevent children with CP/CU+ from performing reparative actions after transgressions and thus lead to continued behavioural problems. This study also suggests, albeit in a less clear-cut fashion, that CP/CU- may be associated with excessive shame, which could result in avoidance of addressing ones disruptive behaviours. These results have implications for interventions that address disruptive behaviours. As children with CP are a heterogeneous group with different pathways leading to disruptive behaviours, the most successful types of intervention will be those that are individualised for the particular risky behaviour that the child is displaying, targeting specific information and affective processing styles. For example, interventions for CP/CU+ should capitalise on socially acceptable rewards and seek to boost the weak ability to empathise. Conversely, CP/CU- children may benefit from school programmes that teach about controlling emotions and managing aggression. Disruptive behaviours at school: Information processing differentiators of callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems Chloe Booth Developmental Risk & Resilience Unit, Department of Psychology & Language Sciences, UCL Literature cited For further information Please contact or More information on this and related projects can be obtained from the Developmental Risk and Resilience Unit website: This poster can be found online at Outlier inspection of the QSE data revealed two participants who had an extreme score of +/- 3 standard deviations from their group mean. These individuals were removed from the analysis, leaving 20 participants in CP/CU+ group, 17 participants in CP/CU- group, and 27 participants in the comparison group. Mixed model ANOVA was conducted on the TOSCA measure: There was a significant main effect of group (F(2, 61)=4.34 p<0.05). There was a significant main effect of emotion (F(1, 61)=12.15 p<0.001). However there was no statistically significant group x emotion interaction (F(2, 61)=0.253 p=0.777). Planned comparisons, corrected for multiple comparisons (See Fig. 2): Guilt AB/CU+ group scored less than the comparison group for guilt (t(45)=-2.958, p<0.01; d=0.84). CP/CU- individuals did not score significantly differently on guilt items than the comparison group (p=.76). Shame For shame items, there were no significant differences between CP/CU+ and comparisons (p=.11). There was no significant differences between CP/CU- and comparisons (p=.68). Similar to the TOSCA findings, CP/CU+ individuals showed less guilt responding, but CP/CU- individuals did not differ significantly from comparison group on the QSE measure. However, for shame items, there were no significant differences between any of the groups on the QSE measure. It is unclear what drives the discrepant finding for shame in CP/CU- group in the two tasks. It could be that the TOSCA task is more sensitive in measuring excessive shame attributions because it does not directly ask about feeling ashamed. Burnett, S. (2006). The Questionnaire of Subjective Emotions. Unpublished rating scale, University College London. Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, Frick, P. J. (2003). The Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits. Unpublished rating scale, University of New Orleans. Frick, P. J. & Viding, E. (2009). Antisocial behavior from a developmental psychopathology perspective. Development and Psychopathology 21, Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. (1997). Adolescent Symptom Inventory 4: Screening Manual. New York: Checkmate Plus. Tangney, J. P. (1998). How does guilt differ from shame? In J. Bybee (Ed.), Guilt and children (pp. 1-17). San Diego: Academic Press. Tangney, J. P., Dearing, R., Wagner, P. E., & Gramzow, R. (2000). The Test of Self- Conscious Affect – 3 (TOSCA-3). Fairfax, VA: George Mason University. Viding, E., Simmonds, E., Petrides, K. V., Frederickson, N. (2009). The contribution of callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems to bullying in early adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 50(4), Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children - Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Figure 1. Scores by group on Guilt and Shame items from the Test Of Self-Conscious Affect measure. Note: CP/CU+ = Conduct Problems with Callous-Unemotional traits; CP/CU- = Conduct Problems without Callous-Unemotional Traits. Figure 2. Scores by group on Guilt and Shame items from the Questionnaire of Subjective Emotions. Note: CP/CU+ = Conduct Problems with Callous-Unemotional traits CP/CU- = Conduct Problems without Callous-Unemotional Traits.