Adolescent mothers’ interactions with their children from infancy through age 5: Continuity, antecedents, and consequences Keri Weed, Deborah Keogh, John.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Introduction to the Environment Rating Scales
Advertisements

Why are you here? REALLY…...
Judith J. Carta, Ph.D. Juniper Gardens Children’s Project University of Kansas with help from Elaine Carlson, WESTAT.
Maternal Psychological Control: Links to Close Friendship and Depression in Early Adolescence Heather L. Tencer Jessica R. Meyer Felicia D. Hall University.
Marital Satisfaction and the Development of Autonomy and Close Friendships in Early Adolescence Jessica R. Meyer L. Wrenn Thompson Kathleen Boykin McElhaney.
Adolescent Attachment to Parents: Predicting Later Adolescent Rejection Sensitivity I would like to thank the William T. Grant Foundation, Spencer Foundation,
Social inclusion of young children with Autistic Spectrum Disorder in Australian early childhood programs Sue Walker and Donna Berthelsen Queensland University.
Abstract Investigated the mother-child relationship and adolescents’ emotional symptoms and adaptive skills reported by youth. Negative mother-child relationship.
 Social & Environmental Variables The effects of SES and Parenting on Cognitive Development.
Julie C. Dunsmore, Jordan A. Booker, Na’ama Atzaba-Poria, Sarah Ryan, Ross W. Greene, & Thomas H. Ollendick Introduction Participants and Procedures Oppositional.
Social Competence and Adjustment in Chinese and North American Children: A Contextual-Developmental Perspective Xinyin Chen University of Western Ontario.
The Effects of Enhanced Milieu Teaching on Children with Down Syndrome
The Discrepancy-Depression Association: Gender and Grade Differences Erin N. Stevens, M. C. Lovejoy, & Laura D. Pittman Northern Illinois University Introduction:
Ten Year Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Mothers and their Children Catholic Family Service of Calgary Louise Dean Centre Holly Charles & Brenda Simpson.
School Readiness Initiatives: Assessing Their Yield Craig T. Ramey, Ph.D. & Sharon L. Ramey, Ph.D. Georgetown University Center on Health and Education.
Stability of Resilience in Children of Adolescent Mothers Keri Weed University of South Carolina Aiken Deborah Keogh, and John Borkowski University of.
Pubertal Timing and Tempo: Associations with Friends’ Behavior in Early and Middle Adolescence Catherine Thompson 1, Meredith Henry 2, Sylvie Mrug 2 1.
Ramey & Ramey (1998) Early Intervention: activities designed to enhance a young child’s development Initial evaluation of child’s abilities and needs (in.
Links to Positive Parenting among African American and Hispanic American Low-Income Mothers Laura D. Pittman Psychology Department Northern Illinois University.
Ecological Determinants of School Readiness: A Longitudinal Study Mark Wade, Dillon Browne, Heather Prime, & Jennifer Jenkins Department of Human Development.
ABSTRACT METHODS RESULTS CONCLUSION Background: Georgia rates the quality of early childcare learning centers using a tiered quality improvement system.
1 Quality of Play: Progress and Challenges. 2 Introduction Key Sure Start objective: Improving the ability to learn Core Sure Start service: Support for.
Adolescent Romantic Relationships and Depressive Symptoms: The Moderating Effect of Emotional Intelligence Introduction David E. Szwedo
Printed by Parent-Adolescent Relationship Quality and the Development of Romantic Values Jessica K. Winkles, Joseph P. Allen University.
Child Abuse Potential and Maternal Perceptions of Infant Temperament Melissa D. Swartzmiller, Katherine L. Guyon, and Alissa C. Huth-Bocks Eastern Michigan.
Abecedarian Project. Problems With Prior Research few early childhood programs have been sufficiently well controlled to permit scientists to evaluate.
Ryoichi J. P. Noguchi, M.S., Michael M. Knepp, M.S., & Thomas H. Ollendick, Ph.D. INTRODUCTION METHOD  Studies of attention and memory have consistently.
+ Third Party Evaluation – Interim Report Presentation for Early Childhood Advisory Council December 19, 2013.
Can Peer Pressure Be A Good Thing? Megan M. Schad, Meredyth A. Evans, David E. Szwedo, Joseph P. Allen University of Virginia We would like to thank the.
High Narcissism and Low Self-Esteem as Risk Factors for the Development of Conduct Problems and Aggression in Children Kristy K. Adler 1, Christopher T.
The Broader Context of Relational Aggression in Adolescent Romantic Relationships Megan M. Schad, David E. Szwedo, Amanda Hare, Jill Antonishak, Joseph.
NAEYC Developmentally Appropriate Practice in Early Childhood Programs Key Messages and Implication.
Early Adolescent Behaviors in Disagreement with Best Friend Predictive of Later Emotional Repair Abilities Lauren Cannavo, Elenda T. Hessel, Joseph S.
Temperament Constitutionally based individual differences in behavioral characteristics that are relatively consistent across situations and over time.
MethodIntroductionResults Discussion Factors Affecting Psychosocial Functioning in Serious Mental Illness and Implications for Treatment Jason E. Vogler,
Youth violence exposure, adolescent delinquency and anxiety, and the potential mediating role of sleep problems during middle childhood Chelsea M. Weaver.
Early Adolescence Social Withdrawal as a Predictor of Late Adolescence Autonomy and Relatedness with Romantic Partners. Elenda T. Hessel, Megan M. Schad,
Effect of Therapist Process Variables on Treatment Outcome for Parent-Child Interaction Therapy Michelle D. Harwood, B.S. and Sheila M. Eyberg, Ph.D. Department.
What is Development? Systematic changes and continuities –In the individual –Between conception and death “Womb to Tomb” Three broad domains –Physical,
Results Time 2 (Age 18-20) Target teen and their romantic partner engaged in an 8 minute hypothetical disagreement task interaction. Hostile, relationship-undermining.
Adolescent attachment state-of-mind and parent-adolescent relationship quality: Joint longitudinal predictors of social and emotional functioning Kathleen.
Adolescent Peer and Romantic Predictors of Youths’ Emotion Regulation in Early Adulthood Introduction David E. Szwedo David E. Szwedo,
Conclusions  These results suggest that the relationship between mother pain catastrophizing and the provision of negative attention and activity restriction.
From Managing Emotions to Improving Relationships: Higher Quality Best Friendships Predicted from Earlier Emotion Regulation. Elenda T. Hessel, Megan M.
Introduction Results: Moderated Mediation The Immigrant Paradox: Associations Between Latino Kindergartners’ Reading and Mathematics Skills, Parents’ Knowledge.
Math, Reading, Writing Growth and Wellbeing in Adolescents Kevin L. Gess The Citadel Reading, Writing, and Math play a big part in a young student’s life.
Discussion Results Introduction From Lateral to Leader: A Study of Preschoolers’ Relationships with Peers Erin Podgorski & Dr. Carin L. Neitzel, The University.
The Benefits of Regulating Emotions: Predictions from Adolescent Emotional Repair to Multiple Domains of Young Adult Adjustment. Elenda T. Hessel, Megan.
Romantic Partners Promotion of Autonomy and Relatedness in Adolescence as a Predictor of Young Adult Emotion Regulation. Elenda T. Hessel, Emily L. Loeb,
1 A Comparison of Motor Delays in Young Children: Autism Spectrum Disorder, Developmental Delay, and Developmental Concerns Beth Provost, Brian R. Lopez,
Physical Discipline and Socioemotional Development in Low-Income Ethnic Minority Preschoolers: The Moderating Role of Maternal Parenting Qualities Ericka.
Development of Physical Aggression: Exploring the Relationship with Language Elizabeth Anson MS Kimberly Sidora-Arcoleo PhD Robert Cole PhD Harriet Kitzman.
WHAT IS INTELLIGENCE? l MYTH: Each individual’s capacity to think & solve problems, including learning, memory, reasoning is: Ü -innate, genetically determined.
Maternal Insensitivity in Childhood Predicts Greater Electrodermal Reactivity During Adult Romantic Conflict Jeffry Simpson, Lee Raby, Glenn Roisman, Andrew.
Longitudinal Effects of Mothers’ Encouragement and Discouragement of Positive Emotions on Children’s Task Behavior Deon Brown, Julie C. Dunsmore, Erika.
Indiana Paths to QUALITY™ Child Care Quality Rating and Improvement System: Outcomes for Children and Child Care Providers James Elicker, Zachary Gold,
The Relationship of Early Intervention and Early Child Characteristics
Mothers' Vocabulary and Autonomy-Granting Behaviors as Predictors of Gains in Children's Vocabulary Competence from Age 3 to Age 4 Sara L. Sohr-Preston.
Adaptive Social Regulation of Emotion and Adolescent Romantic Relationship Power: A Developmental Perspective Erin M. Miga, Joanna Chango, Meredyth Evans,
Parenting behaviors predict effortful control and internalizing/externalizing problems among children during the first year of a cancer diagnosis Emily.
Introduction Methods Discussion Hypotheses Results References
Introduction Results Hypotheses Discussion Method
Negotiating Adolescence: The Importance of Close Relationships for Dismissing Adolescents J. Claire Stephenson, Nell N. Manning, Dave E. Szwedo & Joseph.
Introduction Results Conclusions Method
Introduction Results Conclusions Method
University of Virginia1 & James Madison University2
Introduction Results Conclusions Hypotheses Method
Laura M. Sylke & David E. Szwedo James Madison University Introduction
Puberty Moderates Effects of the Parent-Child Relationship on
Presentation transcript:

Adolescent mothers’ interactions with their children from infancy through age 5: Continuity, antecedents, and consequences Keri Weed, Deborah Keogh, John Borkowski, & Thomas L. Whitman

Introduction Numerous research studies have explored individual differences in parenting behavior, attitudes and styles. Whitman, Borkowski, Keogh, & Weed (2001) adapted Belsky’s (1984) model of parenting to better account for variability in parenting of adolescent mothers. For example, maternal cognitive ability was perceived to play a larger role in variation among adolescent parents, and the role of work a minor role. However, other aspects of parenting research have received less attention. Based on a meta-analysis of studies exploring stability and change in mothering, Holden and Miller (1999) called for greater understanding of interindividual differences in parenting across time, children, and contexts, and of how parents adapt to changing developmental needs. The present study addressed these issues within a sample of adolescent mothers and their children who have been participating in an on-going longitudinal research project, the Notre Dame Adolescent Parenting Project.

The present study had three main goals: 1.to describe changes over the first five years in the interactive behavior of adolescent mothers with their infants, 2.to explain adaptive and maladaptive changes in interactive behavior based on background characteristics and early infant characteristics, and 3.to explore relationships between maternal interactive behavior in infancy and early childhood with cognitive, academic, adaptive, and psychosocial outcomes at ages 8 to 10.

Participants The sample included 92 adolescent mothers and their first-born children who have participated in the Notre Dame Adolescent Parenting Project (cf. Whitman et al., 2001). Pregnant teens were recruited from school-aged mothers programs and from those seeking care at local health departments.  The average age at the time of childbirth was 17.15, with a range from 14 to 19.  80% were in the 10th through 12th grade, with one participant in the 7th and one in her first year of college.  71% were African-American, 22.8% European-American, and 6.5% Mexican- American.  The majority of the participants were from a working class background, with a range from upper to the lowest class (M = 54.5, Range 15 – 73 on Hollingshead’s 2-factor index).  Birth records indicated that infants were generally healthy at birth with few perinatal complications.  53% of the infants were male.

Measures Prenatal maternal assessment  Maternal intelligence was measured with the Vocabulary and Block design subtests of the WAIS – Revised (or WISC – Revised if the teen was under 16). 6-month infant measures  Infant intelligence was based on the average of the Mental Development Index and the Psychomotor Development Index of the Bayley scales  Infant temperament was determined by a composite of the means of five subscales (i.e., rhythmicity, approach, adaptability, intensity and mood) of the Carey Infant Temperament Questionnaire.

Maternal Interactive Scale (MIS): 6-months through 5-years Mothers were videotaped while interacting with their children during each of the data collection periods from 6 months through 5 years of age. When infants were 6 months of age, mothers were instructed to play with 3 toys (a squeeze toy, a rattle, and an infant’s key ring) with their infants for 5 minutes each. At 1- year mother and child played on the floor, surrounded by a variety of age- appropriate toys. During the 3- and 5-year interactions, dyads sat at a table, and were given a simple puzzle, stacking barrels, and either a peg board or shape sorter to play with for 5 minutes. All play episodes were videotaped for later coding with the MIS, an observational assessment that included 10 types of behaviors rated on 6-point Likert-type scales, with higher scores reflecting more positive interactions.  Verbal Encouragement (VE) included 3 items from the Maternal Interaction Scale (MIS) assessing the amount of conversation engaged, mothers’ negative or positive affect, and the amount of praise for the child’s task- related efforts.  Maternal Responsiveness (RS) included 4 items related to reciprocal control, maternal flexibility in adjusting her behavior in response to the child, rate of providing stimulation, and the degree to which she was sensitive and responsive to her child’s emotional state.

Child Outcomes at Ages 8 – 10 All outcomes measures were averaged over the 8 and 10-year assessments.  Child intelligence. Four subtests of the WISC-III (Picture Completion, Information, Block Design, and Vocabulary) measured children’s IQ.  Vocabulary. Children’s receptive vocabulary was measured using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test- Revised (PPVT-R; Dunn & Dunn, 1981).  Reading and math. The Total Reading and Math subscales of the Peabody Individual Achievement Test – Revised (Markwardt, 1989) assessd academic outcomes.  Adaptive behavior. Adaptive skills were assessed with the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale, completed through an interview with the mother.  Behavior problems. The Total Problems score from the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) was used to assess behavior problems.  Temperament was assessed with the Middle Childhood Temperament Questionnaire (Hegvik, McDevitt & Carey, 1980), a maternal report scale. Scores on five subscales (predictability, approach, adaptability, intensity and mood) were averaged to derive a continuous measure of temperament.

Table 1 Intercorrelations, means, and SD among maternal interactive behaviors Verbal Encouragement (VE)Responsiveness (RS) MIS 6 mo n = 92 1 yr n = 56 3 yr n = 78 5 yr n = 86 6 mo n = 92 1 yr n = 56 3 yr n = 78 5 yr n = 86 VE 1 yr.34 ** VE 3 yr.41 ***.47 *** VE 5 yr.29 **.47 ***.48 *** RS 6 m.76 ***.29*.39 ***.29 ** RS 1 yr.29*.57 ***.29 **.45 ***.31 * RS 3 yr.32 **.33 *.73 ***.42 ***.31 **.44 ** RS 5 yr *.32 **.72 ***.26 *.38 **.39 *** M SD Range 1.33 – – – – – – –

Table 2 Intercorrelations, means, and SD among composite of 8 and 10 year child competence measures (N = 92) Child outcomes Intelligence 2. Vocabulary.70 *** 3. Reading.64 ***.59 *** 4. Math.69 **.49 ***.75 *** 5. Adaptive beh-.26 * -.22 * Beh problems-.25 *.25 * *.64 *** 7. Temperament.44 ***.42 ***.45 **.40 *** -.36 *** -.33 ** M SD Range 59 – – – – – – – 4.6

Design and Procedures The Notre Dame Adolescent Parenting Project is an on-going longitudinal study of over 100 teen mothers and their children. In-depth assessments have been conducted beginning during the prenatal period, and have continued through the adolescent years. Assessments were conducted in university laboratory settings, and mothers were compensated for their participation. Sessions specific to the current study are described below: Prenatal: Teens completed self-report assessments related to cognitive readiness to parent, psychosocial adjustment, and social support. Standardized assessments of intelligence were administered 6-month: Many of the self-report assessments from the prenatal session were repeated, in addition to Parenting Stress, and maternal reports of infant temperament. Infant development was assessed with the Bayley scales. Mothers were also videotaped while interacting in toy play with their infants for later coding using the Maternal Interaction Scale (MIS). The MIS included ten types of behaviors rated on a 6-point Likert-type scale. Adequate inter-rater reliability was established by students who did not have prior contact with the study.

1, 3, and 5 years: Mothers were observed interacting during toy play with their children. The MIS was used to code interactive behavior. 8 and 10 years: Maternal report was used to assess temperament, adaptive behavior and behavior problems. Cognitive and achievement measures were determined through standardized testing procedures.

Results 1.How did the interactive behavior of adolescent mothers and their children change from 6-months through 5-years?  Repeated measures ANOVA indicated that the quality of interactions generally improved over time.  As infants matured from six-months to one-year of age, teen mothers demonstrated more sensitivity and responsiveness to their needs.  With further cognitive and language developments between one-year and three-years of age, a corresponding increase in VE was observed.

2.Were maternal intelligence, early temperament, and early infant development related to adaptive and maladaptive changes in interactive behavior?  Regression analyses evaluated relationships between prenatal maternal I.Q. and infant characteristics to parenting in early childhood after controlling for observed parenting in infancy and for SES.  Changes in VE were related to both maternal intelligence and infant development at 6-months of age.  Only early temperament accounted for significant variance in RS after controlling for early interactive behavior.

Table 3 Regression analysis of Verbal Encouragement in early childhood controlling for earlier interactive behavior and SES (N =92) Verbal Encouragement (VE) BSE BF Step 1 Early VE interactions *** SES * Step 2 Early VE interactions *** SES Maternal IQ * 6-mo Bayley * Note. R 2 =.23 for VE Step 1; ΔR 2 =.11 for Step 2. * p <.05; ** p <.01; *** p <.001.

Table 4 Regression analysis of Responsiveness in early childhood controlling for earlier interactive behavior and SES (N =92) Responsiveness (RS) BSE BF Step 1 Early RS interactions ** SES Step 2 Early RS interactions SES mo temperament ** Note. R 2 =.12 for RS Step 1; ΔR 2 =.09 for Step 2. * p <.05; ** p <.01; *** p <.001.

3.Were cognitive, academic, adaptive, and psychosocial outcomes at ages 8 and 10 predicted by maternal interactive behavior in infancy and early childhood?  Regression analyses controlling for early maternal and child characteristics, found intellectual ability in middle childhood was predicted by both 6-month and later VE.  Academic and adaptive competencies were predicted by 6-month VE, but not later VE.  RS during early childhood was predictive of later temperament, even after controlling for earlier temperament.

Table 5 Standardized beta weights from hierarchical regression analysis of 8 and 10 year child outcomes by 6-month and 3 – 5 year observed Verbal Encouragement (VE) 8 – 10 yr Child outcomes F1F1 R2R2 Prenatal Mat. IQ 6-mo temp 6-mo Bayley 6-mo VE 3 – 5 yr VE Intelligence8.76 *** *** *.22 * Vocabulary4.97 ** *** * Reading4.32 ** *** *.08 Math5.75 *** *** **.09 Adaptive behavior5.59 *** * *.36 ***.17 Behavior problems3.25 * * -.23 * Temperament * All df = 5,85

Table 6 Standardized beta weights from hierarchical regression analysis of 8 and 10 year child outcomes by early and later observed Responsivity (RS) 8 – 10 yr Child outcomes F1F1 r2r2 Mat IQ 6-mo temp 6-mo Bayley 6-mo VE 3 – 5 yr VE Intelligence 6.64 ** * *** Vocabulary 3.52 ** *** Reading3.43 ** *** Math3.76 ** *** Adaptive beh4.19 ** * *.24 *.18 Beh problems3.45 ** * -.23 * Temperament2.78 * * * 1 All df = 5,85

Conclusion  Models of parenting need to progress to incorporate changes over time in parenting behavior in response to the changing developmental needs of their children. Our results suggest parenting models also need to integrate different dimensions of maternal interactive behavior over time (Bornstein, 2004). Responsivity may increase earlier in response to infants’ intentional behavior, while improvements in Verbal Encouragement may be delayed until the child has begun to use language. It is unclear to what extent adult mothers, or more experienced mothers, may be more likely than teen mothers to anticipate the developmental needs of their children and to alter their behavior prior to developmental advances.  Longitudinal investigations of parenting emphasize the importance of early interactive behavior for optimal child outcomes. Significant influences remained even after controlling for early maternal and child characteristics. Results provide further support for the value of interventions during the early childhood period.