The Development of Stereotyped Gender Attributions About Emotions in 2- and 3-year-olds: Knowledge Precedes Application The Development of Stereotyped.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Gender Role Development
Advertisements

Social Acceptability of Sport Fan Aggression Based on Gender and Age Jeannie Curry Amy Manning Noelle Smith Martha Young.
INTRODUCTION Research has established that gender stereotypes influence judgments of anger and fear by both adults and children. Currently, there are no.
Sex Differences and Gender-Role Development
The Experience of Emotion in Adults Sarah L. Strout, Sarah E. Bush, & James D. Laird For 2 nd Annual GSC Multidisciplinary Conference, Clark University.
Exploring Emotions.
Problem 1: Relationship between Two Variables-1 (1)
Facial Emotion Recognition and Social Functioning in Children With and Without Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Marissa Reynolds The Pennsylvania.
Culture and Social Interactions, Gender, and Emotions Dr. K. A. Korb University of Jos 1 June 2009.
1 Psychology 320: Gender Psychology Lecture 3. 2 Research Methods 1.What research methods do psychologists use to study gender? (continued)
Method Participants Participants were 68 preschoolers, between the ages of 29 and 59 months of age. The sample was comprised of 32 male participants and.
Designs. Single-factor designs: Between-subjects.
 Your family, friends, teachers and the media affect the way you see yourself.  Gender is directly linked to your identity.
Myles Lynch, University of New Hampshire Boyd Hegarty, University of New Hampshire Nate Trauntvein, University of New Hampshire Jonathan Plucker, University.
How does a cognitive psychologist understand thinking and behaviour? What might a cognitive psychologist say about gender differences? psychlotron.org.uk.
Diagnostic vs. Social Labels: How Do They Influence the Perceptions of Peers? Jessica Bondhus, M.S.E. and Mary Beth Leibham, Ph.D. University of Wisconsin.
Gender Schema Influence Analogical Problem Solving and Memory in Preschool-Aged Children. Gender Schema Influence Analogical Problem Solving and Memory.
Sex role or gender stereotypes. Sex role stereotype This is defined as: – An organised belief about the behaviour, attitudes and characteristics expected.
What is Gender Stereotyping?. Gender -masculine or feminine behaviors - features that are not assigned due to biological sex but social roles that men.
psychlotron.org.uk Greetings, earthlings. We have noticed that there are two sorts of human, women and men. How are they different?
Respondent and Operant Conditioning Together
Explanations of Gender Development: Evaluating Kohlberg
The Expression of Emotion: Nonverbal Communication.
Are Baby Boys Really From Mars? Adults Apply Stereotyped Attributes to Infants Are Baby Boys Really From Mars? Adults Apply Stereotyped Attributes to Infants.
P SYA3:Gender Biosocial Theory Outline the Biosocial Approach (A01). Look at key studies that can be used to evaluate the Biosocial Approach (A02) Look.
Social Learning Theory Explaining Gender PSYB1. Look through the magazines choose just one person you like and explain why…
PSYC 2314 Lifespan Development Chapter 10 The Play Years: Psychosocial Development.
Or not?. Marriage Unit Describe a life event that happened during your “marriage” or “roommate” scenario. How did it impact your budget? Target- Cohabitation.
1 Psychology 320: Gender Psychology Lecture Invitational Office Hour Invitations, by Student Number for October 8 th 11:30-12:30, 3:30-4:30 Kenny.
Cog. & Socioemotinal Dev. in Early Child. The concept that certain basic properties of an object remain the same even when a transformation changes the.
Chapter 12 Gender ED502-Child and Adolescent Psychology By Terri Pardo.
Gender Development Pages Objectives Define Gender Identity and gender typing Compare and Contrast Biological, cognitive and environmental influence.
Method Introduction Results Discussion Different Neurocognitive Abilities Moderate the Relationship between Affect Perception and Community Functioning.
 By preschool age, boys and girls show marked differences on a number of emotional, social, and behavioral outcomes (Ruble et al., 2006). Some gender.
Cognitive Development Theory
An adjective describes a noun or a pronoun. An adjective answers: What kind? Which one? How many? Example: happy dog tired boy seven girls.
STATISTICS STATISTICS Numerical data. How Do We Make Sense of the Data? descriptively Researchers use statistics for two major purposes: (1) descriptively.
Copyright © 2008 by Nelson, a division of Thomson Canada Limited Chapter 12 Part 3 Measurement Concepts MEASURING ATTITUDE.
April 8, Emotional Agent Yao Wang Jian Liu.
Chapter 13: Part III AP Statistics.
The Expression of Emotion: Nonverbal Communication.
A Moral Vocabulary? Exploring the Link Between Moral Emotion Label Knowledge and Moral Judgment in Preschool-Aged Children Meredith A. Henry and Pamela.
Bad Boys and Good Girls? Patterns of Interaction and Response in Whole Class Teaching Myhill, Debra. (2002) Bad Boys and Good Girls? Patterns of Interaction.
An adjective describes a noun or a pronoun. An adjective answers: What kind? Which one? How many? Example: happy dog tired boy seven girls.
Educational Research Chapter 8. Tools of Research Scales and instruments – measure complex characteristics such as intelligence and achievement Scales.
Social Influences on Gender INTRODUCTION:. Two types of socializing ‘forces’ Informal socializing agents People in which close contact occurs:- Parents.
Outcome(s)  Assess the extent to which biological, cognitive, and sociocultural factors influence development  Evaluate psychological research relevant.
Starter – write the correct definition for the following two key terms…. Gender Sex The expected behaviour of a boy and a girl. The physical difference.
Exchange of good practice Gender training material for pre-schools in Denmark Cecilie Nørgaard, Independent expert/Gender- and Educational Sociologist.
RM test. A builder, leaning out of a van, shouts ‘nice legs!’ to a nurse cycling by. The same nurse arrives at work, and casually mentions this to a senior.
Gender Differences and Stereotypes Chapter 4 By: Dan Nourry & Julie Cameron.
A child’s sense of self and inclusion in early childhood settings Karen Stagnitti School of Health and Social Development Deakin University, Geelong. Victoria.
Mothers of Sons and Daughters: Different Influences on Gender Development Judith E. Owen Blakemore & Craig A. Hill Indiana University - Purdue University,
Child Study Language Arts Assessment Portfolio LAE4414- Language Arts for Pre-K/Primary Education Professor Cross St. Petersburg College Shy Taber.
Chapter 18 Gender identity formation. Defining Gender SEX –biological or anatomical differences between men and women GENDER –social, cultural and psychological.
Extension: Could gender be an interaction of the different explanations we have looked at so far? Discuss with somebody else whether you think the development.
Gender Stereotypes in Advertisements. Task On your loose-leaf make a chart similar to the one shown below; for each ad choose adjectives, descriptors.
Sex and Relationships Education
GENDER EQUITY Based in part on How Schools Shortchange Girls. American Association of University Women (1992).
NELSON ALTERNATIVE METHOD Kristie Shitley Nick Seguin Karen Brendel.
Gender.
“Me and My Emotions” Web Quest What other emotions are there besides happy, sad, and mad?
Emotion Knowledge in Maltreated Preschoolers
Analyzing Categorical Data
Introduction to Gender
Complex Experimental Designs Chp 10
% Positive/Neutral Attributions
Cognitive explanation of gender development
Sara Demuzere, Vera Hoorens & Judith Maier K.U. Leuven
Presentation transcript:

The Development of Stereotyped Gender Attributions About Emotions in 2- and 3-year-olds: Knowledge Precedes Application The Development of Stereotyped Gender Attributions About Emotions in 2- and 3-year-olds: Knowledge Precedes Application INTRODUCTION Previous research has found that preschool-aged children’s descriptions of infants are influenced by gender stereotypes. In general, when an infant is displaying no affect, children say that boys are more potent or active than girls. Preschoolers also more readily attribute anger to males and sadness and fear to females. In order to make stereotypic attributions children must both have stereotype knowledge and choose to apply this knowledge. Stereotype knowledge is assessed using forced-choice tasks in which a child must assign one variable to a boy and the other to a girl (e.g., Who is strong and who is weak?). Forced-choice tasks measure knowledge because participants are given no information other than sex, forcing reliance on knowledge of gender stereotypes to answer the question. Stereotype application is assessed using non-forced tasks in which a child is asked to assign one of two or more variables to a specific boy or girl (e.g., “Do you think Rachel is strong or weak?”). Non-forced tasks measure application because participants can choose to apply or disregard the stereotype. QUESTIONS Do 2-year-olds have knowledge of gender stereotypes (forced)? Do 2-year-olds have knowledge of gender stereotypes (forced)? Do 2- and 3-year-olds apply gender stereotypes (non-forced)? Do 2- and 3-year-olds apply gender stereotypes (non-forced)? Do individual 2- or 3-year-olds apply gender stereotypes? Do individual 2- or 3-year-olds apply gender stereotypes? METHOD Experiment 1: 22 3-year-olds (13 boys) completed non-forced emotion labeling and bipolar adjective tasks. Experiment 2: 21 2-year-olds (7 boys) completed non-forced emotion labeling and bipolar adjective tasks and a forced bipolar adjective task. Application Measure: Emotion Labeling Introduced to emotions with schematic drawings Anger Fear Happy Sad Shown 4 10-second clips of a 12-month-old girl in neutral clothing: 3 clips displayed mixed negative affect (sadness/anger). 1 clip displayed neutral affect Children were either told the infant was, “a boy named Arthur” or “a girl named Rachel.” After the negative clips children were asked to label the emotion displayed by the infant verbally or by pointing to a drawing. Karen Singer-Freeman & Nayeli Calle Application Measure: Bipolar Adjectives After the neutral clip children were asked which of 12 bipolar adjectives from 3 categories best described the infant, “Do you think Rachel is…” Potency and Activity adjectives were used to calculate overall schematicity for individual children. Potency Activity Evaluation (no effects found) big or little fast or slowhappy or sad mad or scared loud or quietnice or mean strong or weak awake or sleepy smart or dumb hard or softfun to play with or not… good or bad Knowledge Measure: Bipolar Adjectives Children were shown photographs of 2 infants identified as, “a boy named Eric and a girl named Debbie” and were asked to assign the bipolar adjectives to these infants, “Who is ________ and who is ________.” RESULTS Emotion Labeling We found that 3-year-olds but not 2-year-olds applied stereotypes: 3- year-olds who believed the infant was male were more likely to label him as angry and marginally less likely to label him as afraid than 3- year-olds who believed the infant was female. Bipolar Adjectives 2-year-olds demonstrated stereotype knowledge but not application assigning potency and activity adjectives stereotypically in the forced (65%) but not in the non-forced task (51%). 3-year-olds demonstrated application responding stereotypically in the non-forced task (65%). Individual Differences Although 2-year-olds were not applying gender stereotypes as a group, we were interested in whether individual 2-year-olds who were more schematized might apply stereotypes. We calculated a measure of individual schematicity by averaging the total number of bipolar adjectives that were assigned stereotypically across the two tasks (for 2-year-olds) or in the Application Task (for 3-year-olds). Using these measures we found: 2-year-olds in the Rachel condition who were more schematized were more likely to label ambiguous emotions as sadness and marginally less likely to label them as anger than 2-year-olds who were less schematized. 3-year-olds in the Arthur condition who were more schematized were marginally more likely to label ambiguous emotions as anger than 3- year-olds who were less schematized.DISCUSSION 2-year-olds have knowledge of gender stereotypes but do not apply these stereotypes: Knowledge of stereotypes precedes application. 3-year-olds apply stereotypes to inferences about emotions and traits. Thus, it is likely that they also have knowledge of these stereotypes. Highly schematized 2-year-olds apply stereotypes about sadness and anger to girls more than less schematized 2-year-olds. Highly schematized 3-year-olds apply stereotypes about anger to boys more frequently than less schematized children. Overall, we saw fewer effects of schematization in 3-year-olds. This may be the result of the use of a weaker measure of schematization. Alternatively, it may reflect the fact that there is less individual variability in the use of stereotypes by 3-year-olds.