Investigating the Step Size in a Progressive-Ratio Schedule of Reinforcement for Young Children Diagnosed with Autism Kathryn R. Glodowski, Chelsea B.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Program Evaluation of Music Therapy: Social Joint Attention Behaviors In Preschool-Aged Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Karen L. Herzel, Brenda.
Advertisements

Participants and Setting Study participants included seven children diagnosed with an ASD who were currently enrolled in an early intensive behavioral.
Using Preference and Reinforcer Assessments in Clinic, School, and Home Settings Laura Grow, Ph.D., BCBA-D.
Social Learning / Imitation
Using Multiple Baseline Designs to Demonstrate the Efficacy of Using Behavior Therapy to Teach Children Vocal Imitation Jeffrey R. Miller, Katie M. Wiskow,
Using Multiple Baseline Designs to Demonstrate the Efficacy of Using Behavior Therapy to Teach Children to Answer Questions Kathryn R. Haugle, Chelsea.
Chapter 9 Organizing and Using Data. Using Data behavior therapy uses data to plan and evaluate the effectiveness of interventions current data on antecedents,
Assessing Preference for Attention in a Child Diagnosed with Autism Jodi Ogle, Cierra Micke, Kelly Paulson, Carrie Haessly, Kevin Schlichenmeyer, Matt.
Gili Rechany M.A. BCBA Shema Kolainu – Hear Our Voices January 6, 2005
Common Properties of Differential Reinforcement A target behavior performed in the presence of a particular stimulus is reinforced. The same behavior is.
Direct Behavior Rating: An Assessment and Intervention Tool for Improving Student Engagement Class-wide Rose Jaffery, Lindsay M. Fallon, Sandra M. Chafouleas,
Teaching Children With Autism To Follow Activity Schedules on an iPad 3 Using Manual Prompts and Edible Reinforcement Mark Mautone 1, Kenneth F. Reeve.
Script Fading Andrea Gonzalez Caldwell College Patricia J. Krantz, & Lynn E. McClannahan. (1993). Teaching Children with Autism to Initiate to Peers: Effects.
LECT 5 1 TREATMENT INTEGRITY (Peterson et al., 1982) Treatment integrity: Was the ___ implemented as the experimenter _____________ it to be? Double standard.
Staff Development ED 571: School and Home-Based Programs for Children with Autism.
Using a combined blocking procedure to teach color discrimination to a child with autism Gladys Williams, Luis Antonio Perez-Gonzalez, & Anna Beatriz Muller.
Assessing and Programming Generalized Behavioral Reduction Across Multiple Stimulus Parameters: A Review Megan Duffy Caldwell College.
Antecedent Analysis to Enhance Social Interactions in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Antecedent Analysis to Enhance Social Interactions in Children.
Parent Tutoring (PT) An Individualized Tier 3 Intervention for Students with Reading Problems Study 1 Duvall, Delquadri, Elliott & Hall (1992) Study 2.
 Also called Differentiation or IRT schedules.  Usually used with reinforcement  Used where the reinforcer depends BOTH on time and the number of reinforcers.
Catherine Taylor Caldwell College Graduate Programs in Applied Behavior Analysis.
Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved. Doing Research in Behavior Modification Chapter 22.
A Project GATORSS: A comparison of perceived functions in naturalistic observations and functions identified via functional analysis Elizabeth L.W. McKenney,
Introduction Results Increasing Eye Contact in Children with Autism Alia F. Groth, Tina M. Franzke, & Kevin P. Klatt University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire.
The Effect of Prompting Procedures on the Acquisition, Maintenance and Generalization of Intraverbal Behavior Jennifer L. Jorandby, Stephany K. Reetz,
A Project GATORSS: Social Skills Assessment and Intervention for Young Children with Autism Maureen A. Conroy, Ph.D., Crystal N. Ladwig, Ph.D., Brian A.
Chapter 13: Schedules of Reinforcement
Do jobs identified as high and low preference in a video assessment correspond with high levels of performance on the jobs identified?
Amanda Verriden, Kathryn Glodowski, Jennifer Jorandby, Chelsea Hedquist, Elizabeth Kooistra, Stephany Reetz, Jeff Miller and Dr. Kevin Klatt (Psychology.
Acknowledgments: Data for this study were collected as part of the CIHR Team: GO4KIDDS: Great Outcomes for Kids Impacted by Severe Developmental Disabilities.
Chapter 6 Application of Withdrawal Design. A-B-A Design The Study: Teaching Socially Valid Social Interaction Responses to Students with Severe Disabilities.
Single Subject Research (Richards et al.) Chapter 8.
Printed by INTRODUCTION PARTICIPANTS AND SETTING Ann F. Filer, M/Ed., BCBA John Ward-Horner, Ph.D., BCBA-D Robert K Ross, EdD., BCBA-D.
Training Undergraduate Students to Implement Brief Experimental Analysis as Part of an After-School Reading Program Karissa Danes, Kaitlin O’Shea, Kimberlee.
Investigating the Use of a Blocked Trial Procedure to Facilitate Conditional Discriminations Nicholas K. Reetz, Paula Petit, Sarah Camp, Valerie VanTussi,
Temporal Discounting of Various Items to Examine Characteristics that Affect Rate of Discounting Kathryn R. Haugle, Rochelle R. Smits, & Daniel D. Holt.
Evaluation of Three Leash Training Approaches with Canines Holly S. Perszyk, Jaime R. Barth, Katie M. Wiskow, Jeffrey R. Miller, Tory L. Miller, Brianne.
Evaluating the Effectiveness of Prompting Ratio Procedures for Canine Skill Acquisition Jeffrey R. Miller, Nicole C. Scharrer, Holly S. Perszyk, Nicole.
A Review of Naturalistic Teaching Models for Children with Autism Matthew H. Newquist, Kevin Schlichenmeyer, and Kevin Klatt  Psychology Department 
Introduction Children with autism benefit from early and intensive behavioral treatment (Lovaas, 1987; Smith, 1999). Although behavioral treatment is effective.
Tourette’s Syndrome is a neurological disorder. A majority of patients seek medical treatment to manage tics (Piacentini & Chang, 2001). Tic symptoms also.
Training Interventionists to Implement a Brief Experimental Analysis of Reading Protocol to Elementary Students: An Evaluation of Three Training Packages.
Method Participants and Setting Three second grade students from two different elementary schools in Eau Claire, WI participated in this study. Teachers.
Investigating the Consistency of Results Obtained from a Brief Experimental Analysis of Oral Reading Fluency Christine A. Schounard, Maddie J. Sutton,
A Project GATORSS: A comprehensive model for assessing and treating social skills in children with Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) Elizabeth L.W. McKenney,
Training Individuals to Implement a Brief Experimental Analysis of Oral Reading Fluency Amber Zank, M.S.E & Michael Axelrod, Ph.D. Human Development Center.
Introduction The authors of this research would like to thank the University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire’s Office of Research and Sponsored Programs for financial.
Investigating the Step Size in a Progressive-Ratio Schedule of Reinforcement for Young Children Diagnosed with Autism Kathryn R. Haugle, Stephany Reetz,
The Effectiveness Of Video Modeling Procedures On Request An Item Behavior Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) RESEARCH ASSıSTANT MELıH CATTIK.
Generalization of the Behavior Sit in Canines to Novel Trainers Nicole C. Scharrer, Jeffrey R. Miller, & Daniel D. Holt Psychology Department, University.
Evaluating the Effectiveness of Shaping Versus Percentile Shaping for Canine Skill Acquisition Jeffrey R. Miller, Jonah P. Streff, Nicole C. Scharrer,
Playground Settings and the Impact of Recess on Classroom Attention Christine Peterson, B.A., M.S.E. Psychology Department Human Development Center University.
Kristina K. Vargo, Kelly N. Paulson, Tasha M. Rieck, Nicholas R. Vanselow, and Kevin P. Klatt (Psychology Department, University of Wisconsin-Eau Claire)
Applied Behavior Analysis for Teachers, Eighth Edition ISBN © 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved. Applied Behavior Analysis for.
The Effect of a Trained Attending Response on Rate of Acquisition in Canines Kevin Schlichenmeyer, Jeffrey Miller, & Daniel D. Holt Psychology Department,
Experimental Control Definition Is a predictable change in behavior (dependent variable) that can be reliably produced by the systematic manipulation.
Temporal Discounting of Various Gift Cards Kathryn R. Glodowski, Rochelle R. Smits, & Daniel D. Holt Psychology Department, University of Wisconsin-Eau.
Preference and Reinforcer Assessments Michael F. Dorsey, Ph.D., BCBA.
Investigating the Use of Video Modeling to Teach the Expressive Use of Personal Pronouns to Children with Autism Katie Lichtblau and Kevin P. Klatt Psychology.
E-LAP TRAINING. Who Can Administer? The Manual does not give guidelines as to who can and can’t administer the E-LAP.
Use of a Modified Changeover Delay Procedure to Decrease Scrolled Responses by a Child With Autism Nicholas K. Reetz, Shantel R. Mullins, Sara L. Daugherty,
General and Feeding Specific Behavior Problems in a Community Sample of Children Amy J. Majewski, Kathryn S. Holman & W. Hobart Davies University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee.
Do Multiple Trainers Increase the Speed of Canine Ability to Generalize a Learned Behavior? Kelsey M. Johnson, Jessica L. Pernsteiner, & Daniel D. Holt.
Within behavioral and economic fields, there are several distinct methods for determining the value of a commodity or reinforcer. Two of these methods.
Project VIABLE - Direct Behavior Rating: Evaluating Behaviors with Positive and Negative Definitions Rose Jaffery 1, Albee T. Ongusco 3, Amy M. Briesch.
Therapeutic Use of Groups superKAT :). Group 3 or more people who are together for some period of time with common goals or share a common purpose.
Free Operant Preference Assessment: FO
Identifying & Enhancing the Effectiveness of Positive Reinforcement
Christina R. Weldy St. Cloud State University
Jeopardy Behavior Essentials PCP and PBS Functions of Behavior Tier 1 Strategies Tier 2 Strategies Tier 3 Strategies
Presentation transcript:

Investigating the Step Size in a Progressive-Ratio Schedule of Reinforcement for Young Children Diagnosed with Autism Kathryn R. Glodowski, Chelsea B. Hedquist, Jeffrey R. Miller, Amanda L. Buchmeier, & Kevin P. Klatt Psychology Department, University of Wisconsin- Eau Claire Therapists who provide behavioral therapy to young children with autism need to identify preferred items in order to potentially use the items as reinforcers in a therapy session. Therapists use reinforcers to teach appropriate and functional skills. Many researchers have previously used a variety of preference assessments to gauge which items the child prefers relative to other items (Pace, Ivancic, Edwards, Iwata, & Page, 1985; Fisher et al., 1992; Windsor, Piche, & Locke, 1994; DeLeon & Iwata, 1996). Research has shown that preference assessments may not be accurate indicators of which items will function as reinforcers under increasing response requirements (Tustin, 1994; DeLeon et al., 1997). More recently, researchers discovered implementing a progressive-ratio schedule of reinforcement results in a more accurate determination of which items will function as reinforcers for children with autism under increasing response requirements (Roane, Lerman, and Vorndran, 2001). A progressive ratio schedule consists of an increase in the response requirement after each obtained reinforcer. Eventually the participant stops responding, and researchers take note of the last completed requirement, also called the break point. Researchers demonstrated the break points indicate differentiated preferences between stimuli (Hodos, 1961). Currently, studies conducted in the applied setting have either used an additive step size or a non-systematic step size with no specified ‘rule’ to the increase in response requirement. Roane (2008) urged more researchers to systematically examine the step size of a PR schedule. There are two systematic types of step sizes past researchers have used, additive and geometric. The current study examined the differences in break points, total responses, total reinforcement obtained, and session length in both an additive step size PR schedule and a geometric step size PR schedule for a young child with characteristics similar to autism. Introduction Participant and Setting. One child, Derek, with developmental delays similar to autism participated in the study. Derek was 17 months old at the time of the study and was receiving approximately 4 hours of behavior therapy at a University based program. Sessions for all participants were conducted in a therapy room (2.1 m by 4.6 m) located at a University based program. Sessions ranged from 2 to 5 minutes, 3-4 days per week, for 3 weeks. Participant Responses. Derek was instructed to engage in behaviors acquired from previous behavior therapy (maintenance) which included two gross motor imitations (clap, point, and tap table) as well as three oral motor imitations (blow, tongue out, and raspberry tongue). Preference Assessment. Researchers selected 4 items to use during the preference assessment based on reports of people familiar with Derek. Researchers set out the items on a table in the therapy room before each session and had the participant select one item. The selected item was used as the potential reinforcer during the entire session. Progressive Ratio Schedule. Researchers implemented a progressive-ratio schedule of reinforcement with either an additive step size or a geometric step size depending on the condition. The PR schedule started at an FR2 each session and after each obtained reinforcer, researchers increased the response requirement either by adding two to the previous requirement or doubling the previous requirement. Additive Step Size Condition. The response requirement progressed as follows: FR2, FR4, FR6, FR8, FR10, etc., increasing after each obtained reinforcer. Researchers terminated the session after the participant either engaged in behaviors alternative to the instructed behavior for 30 seconds, or after the occurrence of problem behavior. Geometric Step Size Condition. The response requirement progressed as follows: FR2, FR4, FR8, FR16, FR32, etc., increasing after each obtained reinforcer. The termination criterion for this condition was the same as in the additive condition. Data Collection. The first author gave the participant the instructions and collected data for three dependent variables. The first dependent variable was the break point, defined as the last completed response requirement. The second dependent variable was the total reinforcement obtained which was calculated by adding either the total amount of time spent with the reinforcer (in seconds) or the total amount of consumed reinforcers. The third dependent variable was the overall session length, which was calculated by starting a stop watch at the time of the first instruction and stopped when the termination criterion was met. Experimental Design. An alternating treatments design was used to evaluate the differences between conditions for the break point, total reinforcement obtained, and total session length. Only one condition was conducted per session with only one session per day for 2-4 days per week for 3 weeks. Researchers randomly selected the order of the conditions with no more than two of the same condition being conducted in a row. Interobserver Agreement (IOA) and Procedural Integrity (PI). IOA and PI were calculated for 100% of the sessions for both conditions. For the additive condition, IOA was 100% for break point, 100% for total responses, 100% for total reinforcement obtained, and 100% for session length. For the geometric condition, IOA was 100% for break point, 100% for total responses, 100% for total reinforcement obtained, and 83% for session length. For the additive condition, PI was 100% and for the geometric condition PI was 100%. Method Results Discussion We would like to thank UWEC’s Office of Research and Sponsored Programs for supporting this research. Figure 1 depicts the total responses in each session for both conditions. Figure 2 depicts a bar graph of the average total responses for both conditions. The total responses in the additive condition ranged from with an average of and the total responses in the geometric condition ranged from with an average of Figure 3 depicts the break point across all sessions for both conditions. Figure 4 depicts a bar graph of the average break point for both conditions. The break point in the additive condition ranged from 6-8 with an average of 7.5 and the break point in the geometric condition ranged from 4-16 with an average of Figure 5 depicts the total reinforcement obtained in each session across all sessions for both conditions. Figure 6 depicts a bar graph of the average total reinforcement obtained for both conditions. The total reinforcement obtained in the additive condition ranged from 3-4 with an average of 3.75 and the total reinforcement obtained in the geometric condition ranged from 2-4 with an average of 3. Figure 7 depicts the session length for each session across both conditions. Figure 8 depicts a bar graph of the average session length for both conditions. The session length during an additive condition ranged from 2.37 minutes to 4.37 minutes with an average of 3.33 minutes and the session length during a geometric condition ranged from 1.43 minutes to 4.73 minutes with an average of 2.84 minutes. The results indicate only slight differences in total responses, the break points, total reinforcement obtained, and the session length between an additive step size PR2 schedule and a geometric PR2 schedule for a child with characteristics similar to autism. More differences may be seen when children respond to gain access for differently preferred items, when required to engage in easier responses and when a PR schedule that is more sensitive to smaller break points is implemented. Future research could examine whether differences occur in total responses, break points, total reinforcement obtained, and session length between an additive and geometric step size PR schedule when using a less preferred item as the potential reinforcer. Future research could also examine if differences occur when altering the difficulty of the response type. The geometric step size could potentially be more useful when examining responding of easier response types due to the fast-increasing response requirements. Future research could also examine a PR schedule potentially more sensitive to smaller break points. A PR schedule that is more sensitive to smaller break points could possibly be a PR1 schedule with an additive step size. Figure 1. Total responses across sessions. Figure 2. Average responses per condition. Figure 3. Break points across sessions. Figure 4. Average break points per condition. Figure 5. Total reinforcement across sessions. Figure 6. Average reinforcement per condition. Figure 7. Session length across sessions. Figure 8. Average session length per condition.