CONCLUSIONS INTRODUCTION METHODS RESULTS Huntington disease (HD) is an autosomal dominant, neurodegenerative disease caused by the expansion of a poly-CAG.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
When zero is not zero: The problem of ambiguous baseline conditions in fMRI Stark & Squire (2001) By Mike Toulis November 12, 2002.
Advertisements

Inhibitory neural activity produces a significant BOLD response in human cortical areas Archana Purushotham, Seong-Gi Kim Center for Magnetic Resonance.
TEMPLATE DESIGN © Learning Effect With Repeated Use of the DynaVision D2 Visual Motor Evaluation William P. McCormack,
Detecting Conflict-Related Changes in the ACC Judy Savitskaya 1, Jack Grinband 1,3, Tor Wager 2, Vincent P. Ferrera 3, Joy Hirsch 1,3 1.Program for Imaging.
Charles Pierrot-Deseilligny Dan Milea René M. Müri Eun H. Kim February 21, 2008 Eye Movement Control by the Cerebral Cortex.
Spatial Neglect and Attention Networks
An fMRI investigation of covertly and overtly produced mono- and multisyllabic words. Shuster LI, Lemieux SK. Brain and Language 93 (2005):20-31.
THE BRAIN’S CONTROL OF HORIZONTAL SACCADIC EYE MOVEMENTS Shirley H. Wray, M.D., Ph.D.
Brian White, Karl Gegenfurtner & Dirk Kerzel Random noise textures as visual distractors.
Global and Regional Brain Morphology in Subjects with Huntington’s Disease Prior to Diagnosis Peg C. Nopoulos 1,2,4, Hans J. Johnson 1, Vincent A. Magnotta.
Attention Modulates Responses in the Human Lateral Geniculate Nucleus Nature Neuroscience, 2002, 5(11): Presented by Juan Mo.
Neural Activation and Attention Bias to Emotional Faces in Autism Spectrum Disorders S.J. Weng, H. Louro, S.J. Peltier, J. Zaccagnini, L.I. Dayton, P.
Task 3 conditions Participants decided whether two pictures both matched the cue or not. Event-related design, 40 trials per condition Items were presented.
Susceptibility Induced Loss of Signal: Comparing PET and fMRI on a Semantic Task Devlin et al. (in press)
RESULTS The masking paradigm worked well (participants reported that they knew there was something in the prime position, but where unable to identify.
Stimulus Visibility Reflected in Microsaccade Activity Jie Cui 1, Melanie Wilke 2, Nikos Logothetis 3, David Leopold 2, Hualou Liang 1 1 School of Health.
Methods Inhibition of Return was used as a marker of attention capture.  After attention goes to a location it is inhibited from returning later. Results.
 Participants Right-handed, community-dwelling individuals; 16 younger adults (19-28 years; 8 female); 16 older adults (60-82 years; 8 female). Participants.
Neural systems supporting the preparatory control of emotional responses Tor D. Wager, Brent L. Hughes, Matthew L. Davidson, Melissa Brandon, and Kevin.
LNCD Vulnerabilities in Neurocognitive Processes in Adolescence
Studying Memory Encoding with fMRI Event-related vs. Blocked Designs Aneta Kielar.
Age effects on hippocampal functional connectivity during multifeatural encoding Chris Foster 1, Milton Picklesimer 1, Neil Mulligan, Ph.D. 1, and Kelly.
Pattern Classification of Attentional Control States S. G. Robison, D. N. Osherson, K. A. Norman, & J. D. Cohen Dept. of Psychology, Princeton University,
Somatotopy of the Anterior Cingulate Cortex and Supplementary Motor Area for tactile stimulation of the hand and the foot D. Arienzo 1,2, T.D. Wager 3,
Gaze bias both reflects and influences preference S. Shimojo, C. Simion, E. Shimojo, and Scheier 발표 : 생물심리 전공 설선혜.
Visual Search Deficits in Williams Buren Syndrome Montfoort, I., Frens, M.A., Lagers- Van Haselen, G.C., & van der Geest, J.N.
INTRODUCTION ADULT AGE DIFFERENCES IN THE HEMODYNAMIC RESPONSE DURING VISUAL TARGET DETECTION MEASURED BY FUNCTIONAL MRI David J. Madden 1, Scott A. Huettel.
(Example) Class Presentation: John Desmond
Introduction  Conway 1 proposes there are two types of autobiographical event memories (AMs):  Unique, specific events  Repeated, general events  These.
Orienting Attention to Semantic Categories T Cristescu, JT Devlin, AC Nobre Dept. Experimental Psychology and FMRIB Centre, University of Oxford, Oxford,
ANT Z=52 R ACUE - PASSIVE VCUE - PASSIVE 1300 msVoltageCSD.31uV.03uV/cm 2 AIM We investigate the mechanisms of this hypothesized switch-ERP.
Interaction between chronic and acute pain: down- regulation of motivational value for relief from acute pain 589 OHBM 2009 INTRODUCTION Our recent fMRI.
DIFFERENTIAL COMPONENTS OF PROSPECTIVE MEMORY? EVIDENCE FROM FMRI J. Simons, M. Scholvinck, S. Gilbert, C. Frith, P. Burgess By Alex Gustafson.
2 spontaneous ongoing pain rating stimulus ongoing pain rating (affected area) stimulus ongoing pain rating (control area) mechanical stimulus Mechanical.
Introduction  Recent neuroimaging studies of memory retrieval have reported the activation of a medial and left – lateralised memory network that includes.
A Novel Assessment Tool for Alzheimer's and Frontotemporal Dementias Jeanyung Chey 1,2, Hyun Song 2, Jungsuh Suk 1, & Minue J. Kim 3 The Proportional Reasoning.
Introduction Individuals with ASD show inhibitory control deficits that may be associated with the disabling repetitive behaviors characterizing this disorder.
Kimron Shapiro & Frances Garrad-Cole The University of Wales, Bangor
Oliver Sawi1,2, Hunter Johnson1, Kenneth Paap1;
Volume 46, Issue 5, Pages (June 2005)
Todd S Braver, Jeremy R Reynolds, David I Donaldson  Neuron 
Monitoring and Control of Action by the Frontal Lobes
M.N. Baliki1, P.Y. Geha1, R.N. Harden2, A.V. Apkarian1
Two Cortical Systems for Reaching in Central and Peripheral Vision
Representation of Object Weight in Human Ventral Visual Cortex
Choice Certainty Is Informed by Both Evidence and Decision Time
Network hubs in the human brain
Alteration of Visual Perception prior to Microsaccades
Timothy J. Vickery, Marvin M. Chun, Daeyeol Lee  Neuron 
Volume 80, Issue 3, Pages (October 2013)
Parallel Interdigitated Distributed Networks within the Individual Estimated by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity  Rodrigo M. Braga, Randy L. Buckner 
Jack Grinband, Joy Hirsch, Vincent P. Ferrera  Neuron 
Medial Prefrontal and Subcortical Mechanisms Underlying the Acquisition of Motor and Cognitive Action Sequences in Humans  Etienne Koechlin, Adrian Danek,
Dynamic Coding for Cognitive Control in Prefrontal Cortex
Volume 96, Issue 6, Pages e6 (December 2017)
Between Thoughts and Actions: Motivationally Salient Cues Invigorate Mental Action in the Human Brain  Avi Mendelsohn, Alex Pine, Daniela Schiller  Neuron 
Volume 74, Issue 4, Pages (May 2012)
The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain
Modulation of Caudate Activity by Action Contingency
The Normalization Model of Attention
Monitoring and Control of Action by the Frontal Lobes
Predictive Neural Coding of Reward Preference Involves Dissociable Responses in Human Ventral Midbrain and Ventral Striatum  John P. O'Doherty, Tony W.
Repeatability of supraspinal responses to automated,
Social Attention and the Brain
John B Reppas, W.Martin Usrey, R.Clay Reid  Neuron 
Volume 18, Issue 20, Pages (October 2008)
Two Cortical Systems for Reaching in Central and Peripheral Vision
Social status gates social attention in monkeys
Volume 50, Issue 4, Pages (May 2006)
Human Posterior Parietal Cortex Flexibly Determines Reference Frames for Reaching Based on Sensory Context  Pierre-Michel Bernier, Scott T. Grafton  Neuron 
Presentation transcript:

CONCLUSIONS INTRODUCTION METHODS RESULTS Huntington disease (HD) is an autosomal dominant, neurodegenerative disease caused by the expansion of a poly-CAG tract in the huntingtin gene (Table 1). HD is characterized by motor, mood, and cognitive abnormalities. We previously showed that prediagnostic HD subjects exhibited deficits in oculomotor tasks (Blekher et al, 2006). To extend this work, we developed an event-driven fMRI paradigm to examine cortical and subcortical activity associated with the conscious, cognitive preparation to inhibit reflexive gaze in an anti-saccade (AS task. Thirty-two subjects completed the imaging protocol. Twelve were CAG- and 19 were CAG+. Using the UHDRS, 7 CAG+ subjects were deemed with ≥99% confidence to have a diagnosis of HD, and these subjects were excluded from further analysis. The other 12 CAG+ subjects were considered prediagnostic (preHD). The CAG- and preHD groups did not differ significantly for age, gender, or education (p>0.2). The event-driven protocol comprised 4 runs of 32 pseudo-randomized trials of either pro-saccade (PS) or AS trials (Figure 1). Imaging data were collected on a Siemens 3T TIM-Trio scanner and analyzed using SPM5. Event onset was timed with the display of the instruction prompt. A factorial random effects model with group (CAG- or preHD) by trial outcome (correct PS, correct AS, or incorrect AS) was employed. Eye movement data were collected during imaging. Using an interactive program written in MATLAB, each saccade was classified as correct or incorrect. Trials with blinks during instruction period were removed from analysis. Performance was computed as follows for each individual for the PS and AS trials: proportion correct = correct trials / (correct + incorrect trials). Group effects were tested using analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with education, age, and gender as covariates. Representative traces from a PS and an AS trial are shown in Figure 2, and a performance summary is shown in Figure 3. Correct AS>correct PS evoked more activation in both CAG- and preHD. Areas of activation in CAG- included the frontoparietal network and the basal ganglia (Figure 4, panels a and b), while preHD activations were confined primarily to the frontal lobe and the basal ganglia (Figure 4, panels c and d). When comparing this pattern between groups directly, this greater activation in the parietal areas of CAG- could only be observed in a small number of voxels at a low p-value of There was significantly more activation in the head of the right caudate and the right frontal operculum (FO)/insula in preHD than CAG- (Figure 4, e). In preHD, correct AS>incorrect AS showed activation in the basal ganglia (Figure 5), while there were no such areas in CAG. In CAG- (Figure 6, panels a and b) incorrect AS>correct AS elicited more activation in the anterior cingulate, caudate, FO/insula, medial frontal cortex, precuneus, and cuneus, none of which were observed in preHD. The between-groups comparison (Figure 6, panels c and d) revealed greater activation in the anterior cingulate and basal ganglia in CAG- than in preHD. The opposite contrast (preHD>CAG-) did not yield any significant regions. We found that preHD have greater activation in the head of the caudate than CAG- for correct AS>correct PS. This finding may reflect compensatory activation in preHD when preparing to generate an AS. This hypothesis is further supported by the finding of more basal ganglia activation during preparation preceding correct AS trials compared to that preceding incorrect AS trials. Overall, group differences were most pronounced in incorrect AS>correct AS. CAG- had more activation in subcortical and medial frontal lobe regions that are important nodes of the default mode network best observed in an inactive resting state. One interpretation of this finding is that a period of relative inattentiveness during the instruction prompt predicts subsequent AS errors. Absence of this finding in preHD suggests a different underlying neural mechanism might lead to incorrect AS execution. Pursuit of this hypothesis will require studying activation during the period of saccade execution itself. # CAG Repeats Outcome ≤ 27No HD /- HD ≥ 39HD Figure 2. A representative tracing of eye movements (black line) during a PS (left) and an AS (right) trial. The blue line represents the position of the stimulus. The subject performs the PS trial correctly, but fails to suppress a reflexive saccade toward the target in the AS trial. 3 sec 0° 4.5° left 4.5° right PS AS Figure 1. A central circle turns green for a PS trial (left) and red for an AS trial (right) (panel 1). The circle is extinguished as a horizontal peripheral square (stimulus) appears. For PS, participants look at the square; for AS, participants look directly opposite the square (panel 2). The central circle then reappears (panel 3), upon which subjects fixate while awaiting the next instruction. A single task run consists of 16 PS and 16 AS trials. Repetition time (TR) = 2000 ms. Yellow star = correct eye position. 1. Instruction period (2 or 4 s) 2. Stimulus duration/ Response period (1 s) 3. Return to center; wait (variable, jittered) Pro-Saccade (PS)Anti-saccade (AS) Table 1. Number of CAG Repeats Determines Outcome Figure 3. The proportions of correct saccades. p=0.003 IndianaCTSI ACCELERATING CLINICAL AND TRANSLATIONAL RESEARCH p<0.001 k=20 Z=2 Figure 5. Correct AS> incorrect AS activation of the basal ganglia in preHD. No significant activations were observed in CAG-. A p<0.005 k=25 X=0 p<0.001 k=50 X=0 p<0.001 k=50 Y=6 p<0.005 k=25 Y=6 a b c d Figure 6. Incorrect AS>correct AS activation of the anterior cingulate and basal ganglia in CAG- (a, b). The same comparison in preHD revealed no areas of greater activation. Between-groups comparison shows greater activation in CAG- compared with preHD (c, d) in the anterior cingulate and basal ganglia. a b c d e p<0.001 k=20 Z=2 p<0.001 k=20 Z=2 p<0.005 k=20 Z=6 p<0.001 k=20 p<0.001 k=20 Figure 4. Correct AS>Correct PS activation in CAG- (a, b) of the frontoparietal network and basal ganglia. In preHD (c, d), activation was primarily confined to the frontal lobe and basal ganglia. When directly comparing between the groups (e), there is more activation in the caudate in preHD. fMRI activation patterns elicited by saccadic tasks in prediagnostic Huntington disease Jason Rupp, David Kareken, Mario Dzemidzic, Veronique Bragulat, Joanne Wojcieszek, Tanya Blekher, Tatiana Foroud Supported by R01 NS042659, R21 NS060205, N01-NS , MO1 RR-00750, TL1RR a Research Support Fund Grant & the Center for Neuroimaging/Indiana Institute for Biomedical Imaging Sciences