Specific Language Impairment & Cognition: A Meta-Analysis Michael W. Casby Communicative Sciences & Disorders Michigan State University imail:

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences Hypothesis Testing with z Tests
Advertisements

Chapter 8 Intelligence (only)
Confidence Intervals, Effect Size and Power
Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences
Evaluating Hypotheses Chapter 9. Descriptive vs. Inferential Statistics n Descriptive l quantitative descriptions of characteristics.
1/55 EF 507 QUANTITATIVE METHODS FOR ECONOMICS AND FINANCE FALL 2008 Chapter 10 Hypothesis Testing.
Evaluating Hypotheses Chapter 9 Homework: 1-9. Descriptive vs. Inferential Statistics n Descriptive l quantitative descriptions of characteristics ~
Basic Business Statistics, 10e © 2006 Prentice-Hall, Inc. Chap 9-1 Chapter 9 Fundamentals of Hypothesis Testing: One-Sample Tests Basic Business Statistics.
Tests Involving Paired Differences (Dependent Samples)
Copyright © 2010 Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Prentice Hall Statistics for Business and Economics 7 th Edition Chapter 9 Hypothesis Testing: Single.
ALGEBRA II WEDNESDAY, JANUARY 28, 2015 B DAY DRILL: How many students will be taking the SAT or have taken it? What is the scoring range ? What do you.
Intro to Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences PSYC 1900 Lecture 11: Power.
Statistics for Managers Using Microsoft® Excel 5th Edition
Tests of Hypothesis [Motivational Example]. It is claimed that the average grade of all 12 year old children in a country in a particular aptitude test.
Statistical Analysis. Purpose of Statistical Analysis Determines whether the results found in an experiment are meaningful. Answers the question: –Does.
Are the results valid? Was the validity of the included studies appraised?
Aaker, Kumar, Day Ninth Edition Instructor’s Presentation Slides
Chapter 10 Hypothesis Testing
Business Statistics, A First Course (4e) © 2006 Prentice-Hall, Inc. Chap 9-1 Chapter 9 Fundamentals of Hypothesis Testing: One-Sample Tests Business Statistics,
Fundamentals of Hypothesis Testing: One-Sample Tests
Assessment of Mental Retardation & Giftedness: Two End of the Normal Curve Lecture 12/1/04.
Determining Sample Size
Section 10.1 ~ t Distribution for Inferences about a Mean Introduction to Probability and Statistics Ms. Young.
Sullivan – Fundamentals of Statistics – 2 nd Edition – Chapter 11 Section 2 – Slide 1 of 25 Chapter 11 Section 2 Inference about Two Means: Independent.
BPS - 3rd Ed. Chapter 211 Inference for Regression.
Chapter 8 Introduction to Inference Target Goal: I can calculate the confidence interval for a population Estimating with Confidence 8.1a h.w: pg 481:
Lecture 7 Introduction to Hypothesis Testing. Lecture Goals After completing this lecture, you should be able to: Formulate null and alternative hypotheses.
STA Statistical Inference
Department of Cognitive Science Michael J. Kalsher PSYC 4310 COGS 6310 MGMT 6969 © 2015, Michael Kalsher Unit 1B: Everything you wanted to know about basic.
T-TEST Statistics The t test is used to compare to groups to answer the differential research questions. Its values determines the difference by comparing.
Analyzing and Interpreting Quantitative Data
Inductive Generalizations Induction is the basis for our commonsense beliefs about the world. In the most general sense, inductive reasoning, is that in.
Statistical Inference
Sociological Investigation. Question: Is classroom learning or distance learning the best?
1 Psych 5500/6500 The t Test for a Single Group Mean (Part 1): Two-tail Tests & Confidence Intervals Fall, 2008.
Statistical Power The power of a test is the probability of detecting a difference or relationship if such a difference or relationship really exists.
Essential Question:  How do scientists use statistical analyses to draw meaningful conclusions from experimental results?
BPS - 3rd Ed. Chapter 131 Confidence Intervals: The Basics.
Statistics for Managers Using Microsoft Excel, 4e © 2004 Prentice-Hall, Inc. Chap 8-1 Chapter 8 Fundamentals of Hypothesis Testing: One-Sample Tests Statistics.
Lecture 9 Chap 9-1 Chapter 2b Fundamentals of Hypothesis Testing: One-Sample Tests.
Objectives To understand the difference between parametric and nonparametric Know the difference between medically and statistically significant Understand.
Examining Constraints on Speech Growth in Children with Cochlear Implants J. Bruce Tomblin The University of Iowa.
Chap 8-1 Fundamentals of Hypothesis Testing: One-Sample Tests.
An Innovative Approach to Fair Evaluations for People with Cognitive Disabilities.
Confidence Intervals, Effect Size and Power Chapter 8.
Testing the Differences between Means Statistics for Political Science Levin and Fox Chapter Seven 1.
Chapter 10 The t Test for Two Independent Samples
Chapter 6: Analyzing and Interpreting Quantitative Data
© Copyright McGraw-Hill 2004
Aimee L. Arnoldussen 1 ; Julia L. Evans 2 ; Mark S. Seidenberg 1,3 Neuroscience Training Program 1 ; Department of Communicative Disorders 2 ; Department.
The Normal Distribution and Norm-Referenced Testing Norm-referenced tests compare students with their age or grade peers. Scores on these tests are compared.
T tests comparing two means t tests comparing two means.
Lecture 8 – Internal Validity Threats continued EPSY 640 Texas A&M University.
Copyright © 2013 Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Prentice Hall Statistics for Business and Economics 8 th Edition Chapter 9 Hypothesis Testing: Single.
Confidence Intervals, Effect Size and Power Chapter 8.
BPS - 5th Ed. Chapter 231 Inference for Regression.
Copyright © 2009 Pearson Education, Inc t LEARNING GOAL Understand when it is appropriate to use the Student t distribution rather than the normal.
Chapter 10: The t Test For Two Independent Samples.
CRITICALLY APPRAISING EVIDENCE Lisa Broughton, PhD, RN, CCRN.
Some Terminology experiment vs. correlational study IV vs. DV descriptive vs. inferential statistics sample vs. population statistic vs. parameter H 0.
Inference and Tests of Hypotheses
Effect Size.
Hypothesis testing March 20, 2000.
Hypothesis Testing and Confidence Intervals (Part 1): Using the Standard Normal Lecture 8 Justin Kern October 10 and 12, 2017.
Hypothesis Theory examples.
Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences
Elementary Statistics
What are their purposes? What kinds?
Hypothesis Testing and Confidence Intervals (Part 2): Cohen’s d, Logic of Testing, and Confidence Intervals Lecture 9 Justin Kern April 9, 2018.
COMPARING VARIABLES OF ORDINAL OR DICHOTOMOUS SCALES: SPEARMAN RANK- ORDER, POINT-BISERIAL, AND BISERIAL CORRELATIONS.
Presentation transcript:

Specific Language Impairment & Cognition: A Meta-Analysis Michael W. Casby Communicative Sciences & Disorders Michigan State University imail: American Speech-Language-Hearing Association National Convention, 2008 One of the notable criteria of specific language impairment in children is that their assessed cognitive abilities (i.e., non- verbal/performance IQ) must fall within the normal range (cf. Benton, 1964; Eisenson, 1968; Kamhi, 1991; Leonard, 1982, 1987, 1998; Plante, 1998; Rees, 1975; Stark & Tallal, 1981; Tallal, 1975, 1988; West, 1962). However, a critical look at the research in this area shows that while this is accurate, often times the assessed cognition of children with SLI is actually less than that of their normally developing peers. While it may be accurate to note that children with SLI fall within the normal range of non-verbal/performance cognitive abilities, there is, however, heterogeneity, they typically fall below the population mean, and there are notable differences between them and their normally developing peers (cf. Swisher & Plante, 1993; Swisher, Plante, & Lowell, 1994). The purpose of this research was to conduct an objective, quantitative, evidence-based, statistical meta-analysis of the reported cognitive abilities of children with SLI and their normally developing counterparts. For this research, studies investigating morpho-syntax or non- word repetition abilities of children with specific language impairment and normally developing controls, published in either the Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, or the Journal of Communication Disorders over the past 20 years or so, were utilized in a statistical, effect size, meta- analysis of the reported assessed non-verbal/performance cognitive abilities of the experimental and control research participants. Where possible, available data on research participants’ assessed non-verbal cognitive performance were extracted and employed in a meta-analysis employing an effect-size strategy. These data were utilized in the calculation of statistical effect sizes (ES) (i.e., Cohen's d) regarding the assessed non-verbal/performance cognitive abilities of the children with SLI and their normal control counterparts. Presently, the overall calculated effect size is: d = (95% CI = -.91 to -0.71). That is, an effect size of -.81, with a 95% confidence interval of: -.91 to The minus sign of the effect size indicates that the ‘experimental group’ (i.e., children with SLI) fall below the ‘control group’ (i.e. normally developing children) for the domain of non-verbal/performance cognition. An interpretation of these data is that while children with SLI by definition place within the ‘normal range’ of assessed cognitive abilities, they nonetheless score 0.81 standard deviation below their normal controls for this domain; and 95% of the time they fall.71 to.91 standard deviations below their normal controls. Considering the standard score equivalence standard deviation of 15 for cognitive evaluation, this means that the ‘typical’ child with SLI falls some 12 performance ‘IQ points’ below the ‘typical’ normally developing control child. Though ‘normal’ -- they are quite different. Cohen (1988) characterized effect sizes as: "small, d =.2," "medium, d =.5," and "large, d =.8". By that standard, the overall ES of d = found here is very notable. Effect sizes can also be interpreted as the average percentile standing of the average experimental participant relative to the average control participant. For example, an ES of d = 0.0 indicates that the mean of the experimental group is at the 50th percentile of the control group. An ES of d = -0.8 indicates that 79 percent of the control group performed better than the experimental group. In the present research, the effect size of d = indicates that approximately 80% of the control group population of normally developing children scored higher than the experimental group population of children with specific language impairment for the variable of performance/non-verbal intelligence. Further, an effect size is the degree to which the ‘null hypothesis’ -- that is, the ‘no difference hypothesis’ -- is ‘false.’ In this particular case it addresses the degree to which the following null hypothesis is false: ‘Children with specific language impairment and normally developing children are from the same population as regards their non- verbal/performance-based cognition.’ Given the reported effect size here, the population-level null hypothesis of no difference between the non-verbal/performance cognition of children with SLI and those developing language normally cannot be supported. The importance of the research presented here is that it adds significant information to the database as regards SLI. Perhaps most importantly, it objectively examines the question of – "How specific is ‘specific language impairment’?" In that, this research is a critical and evidence- based examination of potential similarities and differences for SLI, and normal development vis-à-vis cognition. It addresses the argument that SLI may not be so ‘specific’ to difficulties in the linguistic domain alone. The results demonstrate that not only are children with SLI different from normally developing children with regard to language abilities, but they also differ from them, though perhaps more subtly, with regards to cognitive performance. Newbury, Bishop, and Monaco (2005, p. 528) stated that “specific language impairment is a failure to acquire age- appropriate language despite normal non-verbal intelligence and otherwise typical development.” The results of the research presented here, clearly call for an appropriate level of reconsideration/reconceptualization of the domain of non- verbal/performance cognition and SLI. At the very least perhaps, it needs to be recognized as an important though conceivably ‘sub-clinical’ characteristic. Even so, they go on to argue that “…we need to use measures of the underlying cognitive basis of SLI in our quest for genotype-phenotype relationships.” Appropriately so, they point out that SLI is a heterogeneous condition clinically, and likely so in etiology, and pathogenesis. It should be noted that, data from Miller, Leonard, Kail, Zhang, Tomblin, and Francis (2006) were not included in this project, as they more exactingly controlled for cognition by specifically matching experimental (SLI) and control (Normal Language) research participants for performance IQ. This research tactic does provide a markedly better control for the variable of cognition in research on specific language impairment. However it may also lead to biased sampling from the population of children with SLI (cf. Swisher & Plante, 1993; Swisher, Plante, & Lowell, 1994). Specific Language Impairment Normally Developing Graphic representation of population effect size of SLI and non-verbal cognition in children (d = -.81 (95% CI -.91 to -.71) Effect Size