Chapter 9 Intelligence and Psychological Testing.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Chapter 9: Intelligence and Psychological Testing
Advertisements

Chapter 8 Intelligence (only)
Myers’ EXPLORING PSYCHOLOGY (5th Ed)
EXTREMES OF INTELLIGENCE. MENTAL RETARDATION DEF: subaverage general mental ability accompanied by deficiencies in adaptive skills, originating before.
Intelligence and Psychological Testing
INTELLIGENCE AND PSYCHOLOGICAL TESTING. KEY CONCEPTS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL TESTING Psychological test: a standardized measure of a sample of a person’s behavior.
Intelligence & Psychological Testing
Chapter 9 Intelligence and Psychological Testing.
Intelligence. Intelligence and Intelligence Testing Module 28.
Chapter 9 Intelligence and Psychological Testing.
Chapter 11: Intelligence and Psychological Testing
Module 13 Intelligence.
What makes us smart? Or not so smart?
 Problems of ________ structure  Series completion and analogy problems  Problems of ____________  String problem and Anagrams  Often solved through.
Unit 11. * intelligence: * aggregate or global capacity * to act purposefully * to think rationally * to deal effectively with the environment * fluid.
Assessing Intelligence. Why was intelligence tests created? Is it better to separate students into ability groups or to have mainstreamed classes? Why?
Chapter 9: Intelligence and Psychological Testing
Module 13 Intelligence. INTRODUCTION Psychometrics –Subarea of psychology –Concerned with developing psychological tests that assess an individual’s abilities,
 Intelligence is a concept not a “thing”. We refer to peoples IQ as a trait like Height. That error of reasoning is called reification. Psychologist.
Intelligence Intelligence: the ability to learn from one’s experiences, acquire knowledge, and use resources effectively in adapting to new situations.
Chapter 9 Intelligence and Psychological Testing.
Chapter 9 Intelligence and Psychological Testing.
Intelligence intelligence: usually defined as the ability to profit from experience, acquired knowledge, think abstractly, act purposefully, and/or adapt.
Module 13 Intelligence. DEFINING INTELLIGENCE Two-factor theory –Psychometric approach measures or quantifies cognitive abilities or factors that are.
Intelligence & Testing
Intelligence CHAPTER 16 LESSONS 16.1 Measuring Intelligence
INTELLIGENCE The ability to learn from experience, solve problems, and use knowledge to adapt to new situations.
Intelligence and Psychological Testing
Origins of Intelligence Testing  Intelligence Test  a method of assessing an individual’s mental aptitudes and comparing them to those of others, using.
Intelligence and Psychological Testing
Chapter 9 Intelligence & Psychological Testing. Principle Types of Tests Standardized measure of a sample of a persons behavior Represent a sample of.
Intelligence and Intelligence Assessment Chapter 9.
Chapter 11 Intelligence. Mental quality consisting of the ability to learn from experience, solve problems, and use knowledge to adapt to new situations.
Testing & Intelligence Principal Types of Tests –Personality –Mental ability Intelligence tests – potential for general mental ability Aptitude – potential.
Intelligence and Psychological Testing
Intelligence.
What makes us smart? Or not so smart?
Testing. Psychological Tests  Tests abilities, interests, creativity, personality, behavior  Must be standardized, reliable, and valid  Timing, instructions,
IntelligenceIntelligence Chapter 11. What is intelligence? A mental quality consisting of the ability to learn from experience, solve problems, and use.
Intelligence sample IQ questions sample IQ questions What makes us smart? Or not so smart?
What makes us smart? Or not so smart?
Chapter 9 Intelligence and Psychological Testing.
Assessing Intelligence Origins of Intelligence Testing Modern Tests of Mental Abilities Principles of Test Construction.
What makes us intelligent?. The ability to learn from experience, solve problems, and use knowledge to adapt to new situations. Is socially constructed.
Chapter 11 Intelligence “Just Think Mr. Thompson”.
Intelligence the ability to learn from experience, solve problems, and use our knowledge to adapt to new situations In research studies It’s whatever.
Chapter 11: Intelligence and Psychological Testing.
Chapter 9 Intelligence. Objectives 9.1 The Nature of Intelligence Define intelligence from an adaptation perspective. Compare and contrast theories of.
1. Which diagram results from folding the diagram on the left?
Vocab Unit 11. = a method of assessing an individual's mental aptitudes and comparing them with those of others, using numerical scores.
Unit 11 Vocabulary Individual Differences and Intelligence.
Chapter 9: Intelligence & Language
XI. Testing and Individual Differences
Chapter 9: Intelligence and Psychological Testing
Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved.
Intelligence and Psychological Testing
What makes us smart? Or not so smart?
PSYCHOLOGICAL TESTING chapter 9
Myers’ PSYCHOLOGY Intelligence Worth Publishers.
Myers’ PSYCHOLOGY Unit 11 Intelligence Chris Dunn Spalding High School.
EXPLORING PSYCHOLOGY Unit 6 – Part 2 Intelligence Ms. Markham.
Intelligence: The Dynamics of Intelligence
Chapter 9: Intelligence and Psychological Testing
Chapter 9: Intelligence and Psychological Testing
Myers’ PSYCHOLOGY (7th Ed)
Module 13 Intelligence.
61.1 – Discuss the history of intelligence testing.
Intelligence & Psychological Testing
Unit 11: Testing and Individual Differences
Presentation transcript:

Chapter 9 Intelligence and Psychological Testing

n Standardization  Test norms Standardization group n Reliability  Correlation coefficient n Validity  Content validity  Criterion-related validity  Construct validity Key Concepts in Psychological Testing

Figure 9.2 Test-retest reliability. Subjects’ scores on the first administration of an assertiveness test are represented on the left, and their scores on a second administration of the same test a few weeks later are shown on the right. If subjects obtain similar scores on both administrations, as in the left graph, the test measures assertiveness consistently and has high test-retest reliability. If they get very different scores on the second administration, as in the right graph, the test has low reliability.

Figure 9.3 Correlation and reliability. As explained in Chapter 2, a positive correlation means that two variables covary in the same direction; a negative correlation means that two variables covary in the opposite direction. The closer the correlation coefficient gets to either –1.00 or +1.00, the stronger the relationship. At a minimum, reliability estimates for psychological tests must be moderately high positive correlations. Most reliability coefficients fall between 70 and.95.

Figure 9.4 Criterion-related validity. To evaluate the criterion-related validity of a pilot aptitude test, a psychologist would correlate subjects’ test scores with a criterion measure of their aptitude, such as ratings of their performance in a pilot training program. The validity of the test is supported if a substantial correlation is found between the two measures. If little or no relationship exists between the two sets of scores, the data do not provide support for the validity of the test.

Figure 9.5 Construct validity. Psychologists evaluate a scale’s construct validity by studying how scores on the scale correlate with a variety of variables. For example, some of the evidence on the construct validity of the Expression Scale from the Psychological Screening Inventory is summarized here. This scale is supposed to measure the personality trait of extraversion. As you can see on the left side of this network of correlations, the scale correlates negatively with measures of social introversion, social discomfort, and neuroticism, just as one would expect if the scale is really tapping extraversion. On the right, you can see that the scale is correlated positively with measures of sociability and self-acceptance and another index of extraversion, as one would anticipate. At the bottom, you can see that the scale does not correlate with several traits that should be unrelated to extraversion. Thus, the network of correlations depicted here supports the idea that the Expression Scale measures extraversion.

n Mental ability tests  Intelligence – general  Aptitude tests – specific n Personality scales  Measure motives, interests, values, and attitudes Principle Types of Psychological Tests

n Sir Francis Galton (1869)  Hereditary Genius n Alfred Binet and Theodore Simon (1905)  Binet-Simon Intelligence Scale Mental age n Lewis Terman (1916)  Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale Intelligence Quotient (IQ) – MA/CA x 100 n David Wechsler (1955)  Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale The Evolution of Intelligence Testing

Figure 9.7 The normal distribution. Many characteristics are distributed in a pattern represented by this bell-shaped curve. The horizontal axis shows how far above or below the mean a score is (measured in plus or minus standard deviations). The vertical axis is used to graph the number of cases obtaining each score. In a normal distribution, the cases are distributed in a fixed pattern. For instance, 68.26% of the cases fall between +1 and –1 standard deviation. Modern IQ scores indicate where a person’s measured intelligence falls in the normal distribution. On most IQ tests, the mean is set at an IQ of 100 and the standard deviation at 15. Thus, an IQ of 130 means that a person scored 2 standard deviations above the mean. Any deviation IQ score can be converted into a percentile score, which indicates the percentage of cases obtaining a lower score. The mental classifications at the bottom of the figure are descriptive labels that roughly correspond to ranges of IQ scores.

Figure 9.8 Laypersons’ conceptions of intelligence. Robert Sternberg and his colleagues (1981) asked participants to list examples of behaviors characteristic of intelligence. The examples tended to sort into three groups that represent the three types of intelligence recognized by the average person: verbal intelligence, practical intelligence, and social intelligence.

n Exceptionally reliable – correlations into the.90s n Qualified validity – valid indicators of academic/verbal intelligence, not intelligence in a truly general sense  Correlations:.40s-.50s with school success.60s-.80s with number of years in school n Predictive of occupational attainment, debate about predictiveness of performance Reliability and Validity of IQ tests

n Diagnosis based on IQ and adaptive testing  IQ 2 or more SD below mean  Adaptive skill deficits  Origination before age 18 n 4 levels: mild, moderate, severe, profound  Mild most common by far n Causes:  Environmental vs. Biological Extremes of Intelligence: Mental Retardation

Figure 9.9 The prevalence and severity of mental retardation. The overall prevalence of mental retardation is roughly 1 to 3% of the general population. The vast majority (85%) of the retarded population is mildly retarded. Only about 15% of the retarded population falls into the subcategories of moderate, severe, or profound retardation.

n Identification issues – ideals vs. practice  IQ 2 SD above mean standard  Creativity, leadership, special talent? n Stereotypes – weak, socially inept, emotionally troubled  Lewis Terman (1925) – largely contradicted stereotypes  Ellen Winner (1997) – moderately vs. profoundly gifted n Giftedness and high achievement – beyond IQ Extremes of Intelligence: Giftedness

Figure 9.27 Estimated prevalence of psychological disorders among people who achieved creative eminence. Ludwig (1995) studied biographies of 1004 people who had clearly achieved eminence in one of 18 fields and tried to determine whether each person suffered from any specific mental disorders in his or her lifetime. The data summarized here show the prevalence rates for depression and for a mental disorder of any kind for four fields where creativity is often the key to achieving eminence. As you can see, the estimated prevalence of mental illness was extremely elevated among eminent writers, artists, and composers (but not natural scientists) in comparison to the general population, with depression accounting for much of this elevation.

n Heredity  Family and twin studies  Heritability estimates n Environment  Adoption studies  Cumulative deprivation hypothesis  The Flynn effect n Interaction  The concept of the reaction range Intelligence: Heredity or Environment? Launch Video

Figure 9.15 Reaction range. The concept of reaction range posits that heredity sets limits on one’s intellectual potential (represented by the horizontal bars), while the quality of one’s environment influences where one scores within this range (represented by the dots on the bars). People raised in enriched environments should score near the top of their reaction range, whereas people raised in poor-quality environments should score near the bottom of their range. Genetic limits on IQ can be inferred only indirectly, so theorists aren’t sure whether reaction ranges are narrow (like Ted’s) or wide (like Chris’s). The concept of reaction range can explain how two people with similar genetic potential can be quite different in intelligence (compare Tom and Jack) and how two people reared in environments of similar quality can score quite differently (compare Alice and Jack).

Figure 9.12 Studies of IQ similarity. The graph shows the mean correlations of IQ scores for people of various types of relationships, as obtained in studies of IQ similarity. Higher correlations indicate greater similarity. The results show that greater genetic similarity is associated with greater similarity in IQ, suggesting that intelligence is partly inherited (compare, for example, the correlations for identical and fraternal twins). However, the results also show that living together is associated with greater IQ similarity, suggesting that intelligence is partly governed by environment (compare, for example, the scores of siblings reared together and reared apart). (Data from McGue et al., 1993)

Figure 9.13 The concept of heritability. A heritability ratio is an estimate of the portion of variation in a trait determined by heredity—with the remainder presumably determined by environment—as these pie charts illustrate. Typical heritability estimates for intelligence range between a high of 70% and a low of 50%, although some estimates have fallen outside this range. Bear in mind that heritability ratios are estimates and have certain limitations that are discussed in the text.

n Heritability as an Explanation  Aurthur Jensen (1969)  Herrnstein and Murray (1994) – The Bell Curve n Environment as an Explanation  Kamin’s cornfield analogy Cultural Differences in IQ

Figure 9.16 Genetics and between-group differences on a trait. Kamin’s analogy (see text) shows how between-group differences on a trait (the height of corn plants) could be due to environment, even if the trait is largely inherited. The same reasoning presumably applies to ethnic group differences in the trait of human intelligence.

Figure 9.18 Asian Americans’ academic success. On various measures of educational success, such as the high school graduation rates shown here, the performance of Asian American students tends to exceed that of other ethnic groups in the United States. More research is needed on the matter, but most theorists believe that cultural factors are responsible for Asian Americans’ academic prowess. (Data from Sue & Okazaki, 1990)

n Increased emphasis on specific abilities  Moving beyond Spearman’s g  Guilford’s 150 distinct mental abilities.  Fluid vs crystallized intelligence n Biological Indexes of Intelligence  Reaction time and inspection time n Cognitive Conceptualizations of intelligence  Sternberg’s triarchic theory New Directions in the Study of Intelligence

Figure 9.19 Spearman’s g. In his analysis of the structure of intellect, Charles Spearman found that specific mental talents (S1, S2, S3, and so on) were highly intercorrelated. Thus, he concluded that all cognitive abilities share a common core, which he labeled g for general mental ability.

Figure 9.20 Guilford’s model of mental abilities. In contrast to Spearman (see Figure 9.19), J. P. Guilford concluded that intelligence is made up of many separate abilities. According to his analysis, people may have as many as 150 distinct mental abilities that can be characterized in terms of the operations, contents, and products of intellectual activity.

Figure 9.23 Sternberg’s triarchic theory of intelligence. Sternberg’s model of intelligence consists of three parts: the contextual subtheory, the experiential subtheory, and the componential subtheory. Much of Sternberg’s research has been devoted to the componential subtheory, as he has attempted to identify the cognitive processes that contribute to intelligence. He believes that these processes fall into three groups: metacomponents, performance components, and knowledge-acquisition components.