Eye-Hand coordination Stan Gielen Radboud University Nijmegen.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Visual feedback in the control of reaching movements David Knill and Jeff Saunders.
Advertisements

Smooth pursuit.
Attention and neglect.
Results and Discussion Logan Pedersen & Dr. Mei-Ching Lien School of Psychological Science, College of Liberal Arts Introduction A classic finding in Psychology.
William Buchser Neuroscience Journal Club April 28 th 2004 Timing of neural responses in cortical organotypic slices.
1 JHU BME Biological Systems II Motor areas of the frontal lobe Reza Shadmehr.
1 JHU BME Biological Systems II Going from vision to action: posterior parietal cortex Descending tracts from the brain to the spinal cord Reza.
Charles Pierrot-Deseilligny Dan Milea René M. Müri Eun H. Kim February 21, 2008 Eye Movement Control by the Cerebral Cortex.
Covert Attention Mariel Velez What is attention? Attention is the ability to select objects of interest from the surrounding environment Involuntary.
Physiology and Psychophysics of Eye Movements 1.Muscles and (cranial) nerves 2. Classes of eye movements/oculomotor behaviors 3. Saccadic Eye Movements,
Matching brain and body dynamics Daniel Wolpert: – "Why don't plants have brains?" – "Plants don't have to move!" Early phases of embodied artificial intelligence:
Spatial representation and coordinate frames in the brain.
Active Vision Carol Colby Rebecca Berman Cathy Dunn Chris Genovese Laura Heiser Eli Merriam Kae Nakamura Department of Neuroscience Center for the Neural.
The oculomotor system Bijan Pesaran April 29, 2008.
Motor cortex Organization of motor cortex Motor cortical map Effect of cortical motor neuron activation on muscle contraction Population coding.
Understanding Perception and Action Using the Kalman filter Mathematical Models of Human Behavior Amy Kalia April 24, 2007.
Sensory-motor integration Bijan Pesaran May 1 st, 2008.
Consequences of Attentional Selection Single unit recordings.
Cortical motor structures. Hierarchical Organization of Motor System.
Frames of Reference for Perception and Action in the Human Visual System MELVYN A. GOODALE* AND ANGELA HAFFENDEN Department of Psychology, University of.
Optimality in Motor Control By : Shahab Vahdat Seminar of Human Motor Control Spring 2007.
What are we trying to explain? Multiple facets of a simple behavior Stephen G. Lisberger Howard Hughes Medical Institute W.M. Keck Center for Integrative.
A kinematic cost Reza Shadmehr. Subject’s performanceMinimum jerk motion Flash and Hogan, J Neurosci 1985 Point to point movements generally exhibit similar.
Active Vision Key points: Acting to obtain information Eye movements Depth from motion parallax Extracting motion information from a spatio-temporal pattern.
Neural control of Movement Laboratory This work has been partially supported by the European Commission with the Collaborative Project no , “THE.
Sensorimotor systems Chapters 8.
Karen Hookstadt, OTR Spalding Rehabilitation Hospital.
Visuo-Motor Relationships: Plasticity and Development.
A human parietal face area contains aligned head-centered visual and tactile maps Sereno & Huang (2006)
The effect of varying the visual context on trajectory planning and perceptual awareness of one’s own performance 1 Psychology and Neurocognition Laboratory.
Visual Perception, Attention & Action. Anthony J Greene2.
Week 6: (March 15, 2011) Auditory Maps and orienting: need for Coordinate Transformations.
Chapter 7. Network models Firing rate model for neuron as a simplification for network analysis Neural coordinate transformation as an example of feed-forward.
KINEMATICS/KINETICS CORRELATIONS OF ARM MOTOR CONTROL DURING CORIOLIS PERTURBATIONS. A. Pierobon, S.B. Bortolami, J.R. Lackner*, P. DiZio. Ashton Graybiel.
Active Vision: Memory, Attention and Spatial Representation in Parietal Cortex Carol Colby Rebecca Berman Cathy Dunn Chris Genovese Laura Heiser Eli Merriam.
Society for Psychophysiological Research
Automatic online control of motor adjustments -P NANDHA KUMAR.
A new neural framework for visuospatial processing Group #4 Alicia Iafonaro Alyona Koneva Barbara Kim Isaac Del Rio.
Cogs 107b – Systems Neuroscience lec0305 –’meta’ motor control “Why, anybody can have a brain. That's a very mediocre commodity.
Summary of results. Reiterate goal of investigation: How general is anticipatory behavior observed by Land & McCleod? Found: Untrained subjects exhibit.
Primary Cortical Sub-divisions The mapping of objects in space onto the visual cortex.
The brain at rest. Spontaneous rhythms in a dish Connected neural populations tend to synchronize and oscillate together.
Voluntary Movement I. Psychophysical principles & Neural control of reaching and grasping Claude Ghez, M.D.
Direct visuomotor transformations for reaching (Buneo et al.) 협동과정 뇌과학 김은영.
Give examples of the way that virtual reality can be used in Psychology.
How is vision used to catch a ball?
Aligning Vision and Proprioception 2 Inactivation of PMv or PMd with small injections of muscimol did not disrupt reaching movements significantly in the.
(Example) Class Presentation: John Desmond
Self-motion perception during off-vertical axis yaw rotation Rens Vingerhoets 1,2, Pieter Medendorp 2, Stan Gielen 1 and Jan van Gisbergen 1 1 Dept. of.
ANT Z=52 R ACUE - PASSIVE VCUE - PASSIVE 1300 msVoltageCSD.31uV.03uV/cm 2 AIM We investigate the mechanisms of this hypothesized switch-ERP.
Visuo-Motor Relationships: Plasticity and Development.
Neural Circuitry underlying generation of saccades and pursuit Lab # 1 - Catching a ball - What to expect/ think about.
©2011 McGraw-Hill Higher Education. All rights reserved. Chapter 2 The Measurement of Motor Performance Concept: The measurement of motor performance is.
What visual image information is needed for the things we do? How is vision used to acquire information from the world?
The Measurement of Motor Performance
Mackenzie Weygandt Mathis, Alexander Mathis, Naoshige Uchida  Neuron 
Complex Movements Evoked by Microstimulation of Precentral Cortex
Inner space: Reference frames
Posterior parietal cortex
Responses of Collicular Fixation Neurons to Gaze Shift Perturbations in Head- Unrestrained Monkey Reveal Gaze Feedback Control  Woo Young Choi, Daniel.
Volume 66, Issue 4, Pages (May 2010)
Neural Mechanisms of Visual Motion Perception in Primates
The Organization and Planning of Movement Ch
Redmond G. O’Connell, Michael N. Shadlen, KongFatt Wong-Lin, Simon P
Xiaomo Chen, Marc Zirnsak, Tirin Moore  Cell Reports 
Orienting Attention Based on Long-Term Memory Experience
Stan Van Pelt and W. Pieter Medendorp
Eye Movements.
Richard A. Andersen, He Cui  Neuron 
Human Posterior Parietal Cortex Flexibly Determines Reference Frames for Reaching Based on Sensory Context  Pierre-Michel Bernier, Scott T. Grafton  Neuron 
Presentation transcript:

Eye-Hand coordination Stan Gielen Radboud University Nijmegen

Why eye-hand coordination ? Visual feedback guidance Exploratory behaviour

Some scientific problems 1.The dynamics of eye and hand movements are very different 2.Dynamics for the eye are constant; inertia of the arm varies 3.Different neuronal structures are involved  how to synchronize ? 4.Different frames of reference: retinal coordinate system for visual perception Body-reference system for coordination of arm movements World coordinate system for planning actions 5.Synchronisation of visual (afferent) and motor (efferent) activity due to time delays in efferent and afferent pathways 6.……..

Questions 1.Is there a common drive for eye- and hand in aiming movements ? 2.How does the CNS deal with visuo-motor interaction if the dynamics of the eye and hand are very different ? 3.How is information about self-initiated movements used in gaze control ? 4.How does the CNS deal with the fact that dynamics of the arm are different due to variable inertia (arm posture, objects) ? 5.Which coordinate system is used in visuo-motor control ? 6.How does the CNS deal with synchronisation of visual (afferent) and motor (efferent) activity due to time delays in efferent and afferent pathways ? 7.If gaze is used to guide the hand along a path (tracking), does gaze lead the hand by a constant time or a constant distance ?

Dynamics of eye and arm movements

Introduction in saccadic system van Gisbergen, Robinson, S.Gielen. J. Neurophysiol 45, , 1981.

Position and velocity dependent force components

Burster cel

Motoneuron behavior

Neuronal wiring diagram Saccadic RT

Dynamics for limb movements Agonist EMG Antagonist EMG

Smooth pursuit : driven by retinal slip Carpenter, Movement of the Eyes, PION, 1977 Solid : predictable target Open: unpredictable target

Eye-Hand coordination in aiming movements

Eye-hand coordination in aiming movements time position

Coordination of eye and hand movements Gielen et al. Exp Brain Res, 56, , eye hand

Hand and eye positions as a function of RT

Conclusion When the eye and hand have to track an object, they are driven by a common drive. The common drive changes continuously as a function of time Gielen et al. Exp Brain Res, 56, , 1984.

Action and perception are coupled: Action-perception cycle Active versus Passive perception

Smooth pursuit : anticipation/prediction Carpenter, Movement of the Eyes, PION, 1977 Solid : predictable target Open: unpredictable target Tracking of passively moving target and of target motion due to active head movements

Gaze in passive and active conditions

Gaze in passive conditions Mean gain for the passive condition for 3 distances (25, 50 and 75 cm), 3 frequencies (0.5, 1 and 1.5 Hz) and 3 stimulus diameters

Frequency-dependent gain for active (closed) and passive (open) conditions Gielen et al. Exp Brain Res (2004) 155: 211–219

Summary Gaze control is significantly better in active conditions than in passive conditions due to anticipation and/or efference copies

Frames of reference in eye-hand coordination

Frames of reference viewer centered: relative to the eye, head, or shoulder ?

Frames of reference for pointing Desmurget et al., J.Neurophysiol In darkness In light environment

Potential error sources in pointing Error in sensory signal (visual, auditory) Error in storing information in memory Error in coordinate transformation from sensory to motor Error in motor command

Constant error Variable error

Pointing to a remembered target Admiraal, Gielen et al. J Neurophysiol. 2003, 2004

Pointing to a remembered target Admiraal, Gielen et al. J Neurophysiol. 2003, 2004

Subjects were standing, in a completely dark room Targets were removed during pointing

FINGER: no frame, LED on finger DARK: no frame, no LED on finger FRAME: frame and LED on finger s target cue pointing frame delay

DarkFingerFrame Pointing results 1.Errors in perception small for azimuth and elevation, but large for depth direction 2.Errors in motor programming relatively small !

Gaze and pointing to remembered visual targets Fixation is on target after target presentation in all conditions

Gaze and pointing to remembered visual targets Fixation drifts away from the subject in radial direction in the delay period. The effect is larger in the FRAME condition.

Gaze and pointing to remembered visual targets Fixation and pointing do not overlap, but orientation tuning is similar Variable error reflects eye- centered fram of reference

Have a cup of coffee and relax !

Pointing to a remembered target after egomotion gaze Admiraal, Gielen et al. J Neurophysiol. 2003, 2004

DARKFINGER Pointing position FRAME Admiraal, Gielen et al. J Neurophysiol. 2003, 2004

DARKFINGER Pointing position FRAME Fixation during pointing Admiraal, Gielen et al. J Neurophysiol. 2003, 2004

DARKFINGER Pointing position FRAME Fixation at the end of step Fixation during pointing

DARKFINGER Average over all subjects Admiraal, Gielen et al. J Neurophysiol. 2003, 2004 For discrete changes in relative target position: Covariation between gaze and hand position: Hand position is biased to gaze position

Conclusions 1.Errors in visual perception are small in azimuth and elevation, but large depth 2.An external frame of reference reduces pointing errors 3.Errors in motor programming are relatively small; main contribution to variable error in pointing is due to errors in visual perception. 4.Updating for ego-motion is incomplete

Pointing accuracy in pathology: Parkinson Disease

Pointing in Parkinsons Disease Parkinson Disease is primarily a sensory-motor disorder Keijsers, Gielen et al., European Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 239–248, 2005

Reference frames for eye-hand coordination For a retinal frame of reference pointing errors should depend on gaze position relative to target position ?

Pointing errors in retinal frame of reference ! Beurze, van Pelt en Medendorp J Neurophysiol 96: 352–362, H, initial hand position; F, fixation point; T, target for movement. B: schematic representation of the paradigm. Horizontal position of the eyes (- - -) and the hand’s pointing direction (—). Boxes indicate location and duration of initial hand position (H, 0–2500 ms), fixation (F, 2500–4500 ms), and target (T, 3500–4500 ms). Visual feedback about index finger position was available until start of movement.

Spatial updating operates in gaze-centered coordinates. Medendorp et al. J Neurophysiol 93: 954–962, Occipital, posterior parietal, and premotor cortex are active for saccades and during pointing movements, retIPS is activated more strongly for saccades than for pointing. The activation associated with pointing was significantly greater when pointing with the unseen hand to targets ipsilateral to the hand. Although there was activation in the left retIPS when pointing to targets on the right with the left hand, the activation was significantly greater when using the right hand (mirror symmetric effect for right retIPS). Each hand is more effective in directing movements to targets in ipsilateral visual space.

Hypothetical flow of eye- and hand-related signals to Posterior Parietal Cortex (IPS) A posteriorly directed stream flows to the retIPS, where relative joint angles are converted into a map with the position of possible effectors coded with respect to the eye. In retIPS, this map, with the effectors initially coded in the contralateral cortex having the weaker representation, is integrated with the map of target location. Visual information about target location, travels along an anteriorly directed stream into retIPS, which encodes a map of target location in the contralateral visual field. Proprioceptive information of each effector resides within the primary somatosensory cortex, in the postcentral sulcus, which somatotopically represents the contralateral body parts.

Eye-Hand coordination in tracking and tracing

Eye and hand tracking in 3D azimuth elevation depth

Eye and hand in 3D azimuth elevation depth azimuth elevation depth trackingtracing

Interpretation of crosscorrelation : optimal overlap of eye and hand traces Obviously gaze leads hand position. How to quantify the delay between saccadic eye position and hand position ?

How to quantify the delay between saccadic eye position and hand position Tramper & Gielen The Journal of Neuroscience 31 (21): , 2011

Tracking Gielen et al. Cortex 45: 340 – 355, 2009

Tracing Tramper & Gielen The Journal of Neuroscience 31 (21): , 2011

Lead time of gaze relative to finger is not constant ! Tramper & Gielen The Journal of Neuroscience 31 (21): , 2011 After correction for saccadic lead time

Gaze leads the finger by a constant distance, not a constant lead time ! Δs : 2.2 – 3.7 cm Tramper & Gielen The Journal of Neuroscience 31 (21): , 2011

Mean correlation between hand velocity and saccade interval (SD 0.10) * * p<0.01 Eye fixations lead hand position by a constant displacement Gielen et al., Cortex 45: 340 – 355, 2009

Simplified model for trajectory generation for discrete aiming movements

Taken from Wolpert, Kawato et al.

Time delays should be incorporated for moving targets ! Δt 1 Δt2Δt2 Comparing signals at different positions in time !

Sensory prediction Courtesy Daniel Wolpert

Virtual reality

Thanks for your attention !