FMRI design and analysis Advanced designs. (Epoch) fMRI example… box-car function = 11 +  (t) voxel timeseries 22 + baseline (mean) (box-car unconvolved)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Hierarchical Models and
Advertisements

2nd level analysis – design matrix, contrasts and inference
2nd level analysis – design matrix, contrasts and inference
1 st Level Analysis: design matrix, contrasts, GLM Clare Palmer & Misun Kim Methods for Dummies
Group analyses of fMRI data Methods & models for fMRI data analysis in neuroeconomics November 2010 Klaas Enno Stephan Laboratory for Social and Neural.
The General Linear Model Or, What the Hell’s Going on During Estimation?
Multiple testing Justin Chumbley Laboratory for Social and Neural Systems Research Institute for Empirical Research in Economics University of Zurich With.
07/01/15 MfD 2014 Xin You Tai & Misun Kim
Multiple comparison correction Methods & models for fMRI data analysis 18 March 2009 Klaas Enno Stephan Laboratory for Social and Neural Systems Research.
Group analyses Wellcome Dept. of Imaging Neuroscience University College London Will Penny.
Comparison of Parametric and Nonparametric Thresholding Methods for Small Group Analyses Thomas Nichols & Satoru Hayasaka Department of Biostatistics U.
Group analyses of fMRI data Methods & models for fMRI data analysis 28 April 2009 Klaas Enno Stephan Laboratory for Social and Neural Systems Research.
Multiple comparison correction Methods & models for fMRI data analysis 29 October 2008 Klaas Enno Stephan Branco Weiss Laboratory (BWL) Institute for Empirical.
Group analyses of fMRI data Methods & models for fMRI data analysis 26 November 2008 Klaas Enno Stephan Laboratory for Social and Neural Systems Research.
Lorelei Howard and Nick Wright MfD 2008
General Linear Model & Classical Inference
2nd Level Analysis Jennifer Marchant & Tessa Dekker
Multiple Comparison Correction in SPMs Will Penny SPM short course, Zurich, Feb 2008 Will Penny SPM short course, Zurich, Feb 2008.
2nd level analysis – design matrix, contrasts and inference
Methods for Dummies Second level analysis
Random field theory Rumana Chowdhury and Nagako Murase Methods for Dummies November 2010.
7/16/2014Wednesday Yingying Wang
SPM Course Zurich, February 2015 Group Analyses Guillaume Flandin Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London With many thanks to.
FMRI Methods Lecture7 – Review: analyses & statistics.
Corinne Introduction/Overview & Examples (behavioral) Giorgia functional Brain Imaging Examples, Fixed Effects Analysis vs. Random Effects Analysis Models.
Methods for Dummies Random Field Theory Annika Lübbert & Marian Schneider.
Wellcome Dept. of Imaging Neuroscience University College London
Classical Inference on SPMs Justin Chumbley SPM Course Oct 23, 2008.
Contrasts & Statistical Inference
Spatial Smoothing and Multiple Comparisons Correction for Dummies Alexa Morcom, Matthew Brett Acknowledgements.
Advanced Designs. Advanced designs and future directions parametric designs factorial designs adaptation designs (fMRA) multivoxel pattern analysis (MVPA)
Methods for Dummies Second level Analysis (for fMRI) Chris Hardy, Alex Fellows Expert: Guillaume Flandin.
Statistical Analysis An Introduction to MRI Physics and Analysis Michael Jay Schillaci, PhD Monday, April 7 th, 2007.
FMRI Modelling & Statistical Inference Guillaume Flandin Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London SPM Course Chicago, Oct.
SPM short – Mai 2008 Linear Models and Contrasts Stefan Kiebel Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging.
Multiple comparisons problem and solutions James M. Kilner
The general linear model and Statistical Parametric Mapping I: Introduction to the GLM Alexa Morcom and Stefan Kiebel, Rik Henson, Andrew Holmes & J-B.
The general linear model and Statistical Parametric Mapping II: GLM for fMRI Alexa Morcom and Stefan Kiebel, Rik Henson, Andrew Holmes & J-B Poline.
The General Linear Model Christophe Phillips SPM Short Course London, May 2013.
The General Linear Model Guillaume Flandin Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London SPM fMRI Course London, October 2012.
SPM short course – Mai 2008 Linear Models and Contrasts Jean-Baptiste Poline Neurospin, I2BM, CEA Saclay, France.
General Linear Model & Classical Inference London, SPM-M/EEG course May 2016 Sven Bestmann, Sobell Department, Institute of Neurology, UCL
Advanced Designs fMRI: Theory and Practice Spring 2010.
Group Analyses Guillaume Flandin SPM Course London, October 2016
Contrast and Inferences
Topological Inference
General Linear Model & Classical Inference
The general linear model and Statistical Parametric Mapping
The General Linear Model
2nd Level Analysis Methods for Dummies 2010/11 - 2nd Feb 2011
Group analyses Thanks to Will Penny for slides and content
and Stefan Kiebel, Rik Henson, Andrew Holmes & J-B Poline
The General Linear Model (GLM)
Topological Inference
Contrasts & Statistical Inference
The General Linear Model
Group analyses Thanks to Will Penny for slides and content
The general linear model and Statistical Parametric Mapping
The General Linear Model
Hierarchical Models and
The General Linear Model (GLM)
Contrasts & Statistical Inference
The General Linear Model
WellcomeTrust Centre for Neuroimaging University College London
The General Linear Model
Wellcome Dept. of Imaging Neuroscience University College London
The General Linear Model
Wellcome Dept. of Imaging Neuroscience University College London
Contrasts & Statistical Inference
Presentation transcript:

fMRI design and analysis Advanced designs

(Epoch) fMRI example… box-car function = 11 +  (t) voxel timeseries 22 + baseline (mean) (box-car unconvolved)

(Epoch) fMRI example… y data vector (voxel time series) = =X design matrix 11 22  parameters   +  + error vector

(Epoch) fMRI example… …fitted and adjusted data Raw fMRI timeseries Residualshighpass filtered (and scaled) fitted high-pass filter Adjusted data fitted box-car

Convolution with HRF Boxcar function convolved with HRF = hæmodynamic response  ResidualsUnconvolved fit Convolved fit Residuals (less structure)

Fixed vs. Random Effects

Subject 1 Subjects can be Fixed or Random variables If subjects are a Fixed variable in a single design matrix (SPM “sessions”), the error term conflates within- and between-subject variance –But in fMRI (unlike PET) the between-scan variance is normally much smaller than the between-subject variance If one wishes to make an inference from a subject sample to the population, one needs to treat subjects as a Random variable, and needs a proper mixture of within- and between-subject variance In SPM, this is achieved by a two-stage procedure: 1) (Contrasts of) parameters are estimated from a (Fixed Effect) model for each subject 2) Images of these contrasts become the data for a second design matrix (usually simple t- test or ANOVA) Subjects can be Fixed or Random variables If subjects are a Fixed variable in a single design matrix (SPM “sessions”), the error term conflates within- and between-subject variance –But in fMRI (unlike PET) the between-scan variance is normally much smaller than the between-subject variance If one wishes to make an inference from a subject sample to the population, one needs to treat subjects as a Random variable, and needs a proper mixture of within- and between-subject variance In SPM, this is achieved by a two-stage procedure: 1) (Contrasts of) parameters are estimated from a (Fixed Effect) model for each subject 2) Images of these contrasts become the data for a second design matrix (usually simple t- test or ANOVA) Subject 2 Subject 3 Subject 4 Subject 6 Multi-subject Fixed Effect model error df ~ 300 Subject 5

WHEN special case of n independent observations per subject: var(  pop ) =  2 b   +  2 w / Nn Two-stage “Summary Statistic” approach       p < (uncorrected) SPM{t} 1 st -level (within-subject)2 nd -level (between-subject) contrast images of c  i 11 ^ 22 ^ 33 ^ 44 ^ 55 ^ 66 ^ N=6 subjects (error df =5) One-sample t-test ^  pop  ^ ^   1 ) ^   w   within-subject error ^   2 )   3 ) ^   4 ) ^   5 ) ^   6 )

Statistical inference

Types of Errors Slide modified from Duke course Is the region truly active? Does our stat test indicate that the region is active? Yes No YesNo HIT Type I Error Type II Error Correct Rejection p value: probability of a Type I error e.g., p <.05 “There is less than a 5% probability that a voxel our stats have declared as “active” is in reality NOT active

If n=100,000 voxels tested with p u =0.05 of falsely rejecting H o... …then approx n  p u (eg 5,000) will do so by chance (false positives, or “type I” errors) Therefore need to “correct” p- values for number of comparisons A severe correction would be a Bonferroni, where p c = p u /n… …but this is only appropriate when the n tests independent… … SPMs are smooth, meaning that nearby voxels are correlated => Random Field Theory... If n=100,000 voxels tested with p u =0.05 of falsely rejecting H o... …then approx n  p u (eg 5,000) will do so by chance (false positives, or “type I” errors) Therefore need to “correct” p- values for number of comparisons A severe correction would be a Bonferroni, where p c = p u /n… …but this is only appropriate when the n tests independent… … SPMs are smooth, meaning that nearby voxels are correlated => Random Field Theory... Multiple comparisons… Gaussian 10mm FWHM (2mm pixels) p u = 0.05 SPM{t}Eg random noise

Random Field Theory (RFT) Consider SPM as lattice representation of continuous random field “Euler characteristic”: a topological measure (# “components” - # “holes”) Euler depends on smoothness Smoothness estimated by covariance of partial derivatives of residuals (expressed as “resels” or FWHM) Smoothness does not have to be stationary (for height thresholding): estimated locally as “resels-per-voxel” (RPV)

DESIGNS

= trial of another type (e.g., place image) = trial of one type (e.g., face image) = null trial (nothing happens) Design Types Block Design Slow ER Design Rapid Counterbalanced ER Design Rapid Jittered ER Design Mixed Design

Parametric designs

An Example Culham et al., 1998, J. Neuorphysiol.

Analysis of Parametric Designs parametric variant: passive viewing and tracking of 1, 2, 3, 4 or 5 balls

Factorial Designs

Example: Sugiura et al. (2005, JOCN) showed subjects pictures of objects and places. The objects and places were either familiar (e.g., the subject’s office or the subject’s bag) or unfamiliar (e.g., a stranger’s office or a stranger’s bag) This is a “2 x 2 factorial design” (2 stimuli x 2 familiarity levels)

Statistical Approaches In a 2 x 2 design, you can make up to six comparisons between pairs of conditions (A1 vs. A2, B1 vs. B2, A1 vs. B1, A2 vs. B2, A1 vs. B2, A2 vs. B1). This is a lot of comparisons (and if you do six comparisons with p <.05, your overall p value is.05 x 6 =.3 which is high). How do you decide which to perform?

Factorial Designs Main effects Difference between columns Difference between rows Interactions Difference between columns depending on status of row (or vice versa)

Main Effect of Stimuli In LO, there is a greater activation to Objects than Places In the PPA, there is greater activation to Places than Objects

Main Effect of Familiarity In the precuneus, familiar objects generated more activation than unfamiliar objects

Interaction of Stimuli and Familiarity In the posterior cingulate, familiarity made a difference for places but not objects

fMR Adaptation

Using fMR Adaptation to Study Coding Example: We know that neurons in the brain can be tuned for individual faces “Jennifer Aniston” neuron in human medial temporal lobe Quiroga et al., 2005, Nature

Using fMR Adaptation to Study Tuning Activation Neuron 1 likes Jennifer Aniston Neuron 2 likes Julia Roberts Neuron 3 likes Brad Pitt Even though there are neurons tuned to each object, the population as a whole shows no preference fMRI resolution is typically around 3 x 3 x 6 mm so each sample comes from millions of neurons

fMR Adaptation If you show a stimulus twice in a row, you get a reduced response the second time  Repeated Face Trial  Unrepeated Face Trial Time Hypothetical Activity in Face-Selective Area (e.g., FFA) Activation

msec fMRI Adaptation Slide modified from Russell Epstein “different” trial: “same” trial:

And more… We could use this technique to determine the selectivity of face- selective areas to many other dimensions Repeated Individual, Different Expression Repeated Expression, Different Individual

Why is adaptation useful? Now we can ask what it takes for stimulus to be considered the “same” in an area For example, do face-selective areas care about viewpoint? Time Activation  Repeated Individual, Different Viewpoint Viewpoint invariance: area codes the face as the same despite the viewpoint change Viewpoint selectivity: area codes the face as different when viewpoint changes

= = viewpoint-specific viewpoint-invariant Are scene representations in FFA viewpoint-invariant or viewpoint-specific?

LOpFs (~=FFA) Viewpoint dependence in LOC Source: Kalanit Grill-Spector

Belin & Zatorre (2003) Neuroreport - fMRI adaptation -14 subjects, passive listening -12 ‘adapt-Syllable’ blocs (1 syllable, 12 speakers) -12 ‘adapt-Speaker’ blocs (1 speaker, 12 words) - Same 144 stimuli in the two conditions Adaptation to speaker identity

Von Kriegstein et al (2003) Cognitive Brain Research Belin & Zatorre (2003) Neuroreport Petkov et al (2008) Nat Neurosci Adaptation to speaker identity

Problems The basis for effect is not well-understood this is seen in the many terms used to describe it fMR adaptation (fMR-A) priming repetition suppression The effect could be due to many factors such as: repeated stimuli are processed more “efficiently” more quickly? with fewer action potentials? with fewer neurons involved? repeated stimuli draw less attention repeated stimuli may not have to be encoded into memory repeated stimuli affect other levels of processing with input to area demonstrating adaptation (data from Vogels et al.) subjects may come to expect repetitions and their predictions may be violated by novel stimuli (Summerfield et al., 2008, Nat. Neurosci.)

Multivoxel Pattern Analyses

Multivariate statistics Traditional fMRI analyses use a ‘massive univariate approach’ -> Information on the sensitivity of brain regions to sensory stimulation or cognitive tasks But they miss the potentially rich information contained in the pattern of distributed activity over a number of voxels.

Data-Driven Approaches

Data Driven Analyses Hasson et al. (2004, Science) showed subjects clips from a movie and found voxels which showed significant time correlations between subjects

Reverse correlation They went back to the movie clips to find the common feature that may have been driving the intersubject consistency