Effect of Cycloheximide on Extinction of Odor Discrimination Learning in Rats Alexandra Knoppel, Katherine Janson, Sonnett White, and Gretchen Hanson Gotthard.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Animal model of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD): Effects of propranolol on exploration in an elevated plus maze in rats G.H. Gotthard, J. Marini,
Advertisements

Reconsolidation Experiment Psychology 4870 Winter 2008.
Protein Synthesis Inhibition and Appetitive Memory Reconsolidation: Effects of Cycloheximide on Retrained Odor Discrimination Memory in Long-Evans Rats.
BNST lesions aggravate behavioral despair but do not impair navigational learning in rats Pezük, Göz, Aksoy, Canbeyli Brain Research Bulletin-2006.
Retrograde Amnesia: Is Memory Gone For Good Or Just Hiding Out? Gretchen Hanson Gotthard, Ph.D. Randolph-Macon Woman’s College Lynchburg, VA.
The Effects of Vicarious Learning with Experienced Rats Jessie Young and Lyndsey Wells Randolph College Lynchburg, VA Introduction Vicarious learning.
Limited Intermittent Access to a Highly Palatable Food Combined with Chronic Mild Stress Induces Overeating in Laboratory Rats Kishan Govind, Robynn Mackechnie,
Experiment 2: Context generalization following delayed context shift.
Examination of the State Dependent Properties of WIN on Spatial Learning and Memory in Rats in the Sand Maze Ashley R. Smith and Gretchen Hanson.
 Left Panel. The two highest nicotine doses (0.8 and 2.0 mg/kg) produced a CTA on several sessions and the test session in the WKY rats. (* = 0.8 mg/kg.
Protein synthesis inhibition and extinction: Does cycloheximide produce amnesia for extinction of an odor discrimination in rats? Alexandra Knoppel, Katherine.
Double Dissociation of Amygdala and Hippocampal Contributions to Trace and Delay Fear Conditioning Jonathan D. Raybuck*, K. Matthew Lattal Department of.
Protein Synthesis is Not Required for Extinction of Paired-Associate Odor Discriminations Sarah Linderman, Elizabeth Nguyen, and Gretchen Hanson Gotthard.
Blocking frustrating memories using the protein synthesis inhibitor cycloheximide and the β- adrenergic receptor antagonist propranolol in an appetitive.
From Mechanisms of Memory, second edition
Rodent Behavioral Learning and Memory Models
Lecture 22: Animal Cognition I (Memory) Learning, Psychology 5310 Spring, 2015 Professor Delamater.
Figure 3: H5 Probe and Control Data Bar 1: Percent correct on regular probe trials. Bar 2: Percent correct on probes with blind exp. Bar 3: Percent correct.
Competitive Place Task
Sleep Deprivation Effects on Memory and Anxiety in Mice Eyitemi Opuogen Department of Biology, York College of Pennsylvania Sleep is imperative for a person’s.
Chapter 10 Aversive Control: Avoidance and Punishment.
Perception and the Medial Temporal Lobe: Evaluating the Current Evidence Wendy Suzuki.
Manhasset Science Research Paris Thatos
Left and Right Panels. Mice treated with prazosin were less active than their vehicle-paired counterparts (left panel). Time course analysis shows paired.
Extinction of auditory fear conditioning requires MAPK/ERK activation in the basolateral amygdala Herry et al European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 24.
Targeting reconsolidation as a new therapeutic strategy Karim Nader Alfred P. Sloan Fellow Dept. of Psychology McGill University Montreal Canada.
CP-154,526, a CRF type-1 receptor antagonist, attenuates the cue-and methamphetamine-induced reinstatement of extinguished methamphetamine- seeking behavior.
Does Scheduled Intermittent Access to a Highly Palatable Carbohydrate-Rich Food Trigger Binge Eating in Laboratory Rats? Robynn Mackechnie, Kishan Govind,
Spatial Learning and Memory in the Sand Maze: Does WIN-2 Produce State Dependent Retention in Rats? Ashley R. Smith and Gretchen Hanson Gotthard Randolph-Macon.
Determinants of Olfactory Memory Span in Rats MacQueen, D. A., Bullard, L. A., & Galizio, M. University of North Carolina Wilmington Introduction Dudchenko,
Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research
Role of muscarinic M1 receptors in inhibitory avoidance and contextual fear conditioning Juliana Carlota Kramer Soares, Raquel Vecchio Fornari, Maria Gabriela.
Disruption of Reconsolidation as a Treatment for PTSD Psychology 355: Cognitive Psychology Instructor: John Miyamoto 05/07 /2015: Lecture 06-4 This Powerpoint.
Speaker :Chi-Yuan Chang Advisor : Yn-ho Huang Ph.D.
Three animals (F3, F6, G13) developed high levels of accuracy and showed rapid acquisition during baseline sessions. Three animals (F3, F6, G13) have shown.
Neuropeptide S Enhances Memory During the Consolidation Phase and Interacts with Noradrenergic Systems in the Brain Okamura et. al. Presented by: Justin.
Learning like a girl? “Estrogen modulates learning in female rats by acting directly at distinct memory systems” L. Zurkovsky et al. Neuroscience (2007)
The Use of Chaining and Reinforcement in an Obstacle Course Vivian Scott & Elishia Webster Randolph-Macon Woman’s College Lynchburg, VA Introduction.
Introduction  Conway 1 proposes there are two types of autobiographical event memories (AMs):  Unique, specific events  Repeated, general events  These.
The Effects of Different Types of Food Reinforcers on Performance in the T-Maze Sarah A. Linderman, Fahima Vakalia, and Laura Revetta Randolph-Macon Woman’s.
Genetic enhancement of learning and memory in mice. Tang YP et al.
“Enhancement of extinction memory consolidation: The role of the noradrenergic and GABAergic systems within the basolateral amygdala” Neurobiology of Learning.
Functional Inactivation of the Amygdala before But Not after Auditory Fear Conditioning Prevents Memory Formation Ann E. Wilensky, Glenn E. Schafe, and.
Cognitive Impairment and Neural Pregenitor Cell Destruction (on Lister hooded rats) as a Result of Chemotherapy Morgan Earle, Department of Biological.
14 The Fate of Retrieved Memories. Chapter Goals Under some conditions, retrieving or reactivating a consolidated memory can return it to an active, labile.
Ventral Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Emotional Perseveration: The Memory for Prior Extinction Training Maria A. Morgan, Jay Schulkin, Joseph E. LeDoux.
Emotion Knowledge in Maltreated Preschoolers
Timing of SCH23390 Administration Influences Extinction of Conditioned Hyperactivity in Mice Anthony S. Rauhut 1,2, Kristen Ratner2, Sandy Buck2, and Ee-Rah.
From: Propofol Limits Microglial Activation after Experimental Brain Trauma through Inhibition of Nicotinamide Adenine Dinucleotide Phosphate Oxidase Anesthes.
Parahippocampal Cortex
BEHAVIOR AND MEMORY EVALUATION OF RATS TREATED WITH SIMVASTATIN IN THE SCOPOLAMINE ANIMAL MODEL TONIN, F. S.; BARBIERO, J. K. Departamento de Farmacologia,
Examination of the State Dependent Properties of WIN on Spatial Learning and Memory in Rats in the Sand Maze   Ashley R. Smith and Gretchen Hanson.
Cellular and Systems Reconsolidation in the Hippocampus
Introduction Results Discussion Method References
Protein synthesis inhibition and extinction:
Maintaining Brain Health
Elizabeth A. Phelps, Joseph E. LeDoux  Neuron 
Vicarious Learning in Laboratory Rats
Randolph-Macon Woman’s College
Fig. 1. APP/PS1;C3 KO mice show improved cognitive flexibility (reversal) compared to APP/PS1 mice at 16 months of age. APP/PS1;C3 KO mice show improved.
Volume 60, Issue 2, Pages (October 2008)
Interplay of Hippocampus and Prefrontal Cortex in Memory
Do Rats Have the Ability to Discriminate Between Words?
Volume 71, Issue 4, Pages (August 2011)
Volume 50, Issue 3, Pages (May 2006)
Volume 23, Issue 6, Pages (March 2013)
Cellular and Systems Reconsolidation in the Hippocampus
Single reward vs. two different rewards:
Maintaining Brain Health
Fig. 1. APP/PS1;C3 KO mice show improved cognitive flexibility (reversal) compared to APP/PS1 mice at 16 months of age. APP/PS1;C3 KO mice show improved.
Presentation transcript:

Effect of Cycloheximide on Extinction of Odor Discrimination Learning in Rats Alexandra Knoppel, Katherine Janson, Sonnett White, and Gretchen Hanson Gotthard Randolph College Lynchburg, VA Introduction Much research has shown that the administration of protein synthesis inhibitors blocks the formation of new fear memories (e.g., Nader, Schafe, & LeDoux, 2000). In fact, most of the research in this area has focused on the acquisition of fear responses or used tasks that required animals to respond under aversive conditions (e.g., Morris water maze; Meiri & Rosenblum, 1998). Additionally, most studies examining the effects of blocked protein synthesis have examined acquisition of a completely new response, rather than extinction of an already-established response. A small number of studies have begun to examine the effects of protein synthesis inhibition on extinction (Lattal & Abel, 2001; Suzuki, et al., 2004); however, the results have been mixed and have been conducted with aversive tasks only (e.g., fear conditioning and the water maze). The present studied used an appetitive odor discrimination digging task (Bunsey & Eichenbaum, 1996) to examine the effects of a protein synthesis inhibitor (cycloheximide) on extinction in rats. Considering extinction is similar to acquisition in that it also involves new learning, it was hypothesized that protein synthesis inhibition would block extinction and produce continued high levels of responding during testing. Results Acquisition There were no differences between the Cycloheximide group and the Vehicle group during acquisition [t(9)=.208, p=.84]. Choice performance during acquisition was statistically greater than could be due to chance [M=90.90, SD=12.61; t(10)=10.757, p<.001]. Therefore, all rats included in the present analyses met acquisition criterion (i.e., at least 75% correct overall during training). Extinction All rats met the extinction criterion in three or less extinction trials (i.e., no digging for 30 consecutive seconds). Testing There were no differences in preference scores for the Cycloheximide group (Test 1: M=.37, SD=.50; Test 2: M=.33, SD=.52) and the Vehicle group (Test 1: M=.00, SD=.00; Test 2: M=.20, SD=.45) on Test 1: t(9)=1.636, p=.136, or Test 2: t(9)=.452, p=.662 (see Figure 2). The Cycloheximide group [Test 1: t(5)=1.808, p=.13; Test 2: t(5)=1.581, p=.175] and the Vehicle group [Test 2: t(4)=1.00, p=.374] did not differ from chance on all test trials (see Figure 2). Shaping (Day 1) One cup of unscented sand (two trials) Shaping (Day 2) Two cups of unscented sand (two trials) Shaping (Day 3) Two cups of unscented sand (six trials) Training (Day 4) Trained on one odor discrimination (at least six trials with 75% correct total) Extinction (Day 5) Extinction trials for the odor discrimination (at least two trials) Testing (Day 6) Two test trials for odor discrimination Figure 1

Method Subjects Ninety-day old, male Long-Evans rats (N=11) were reduced to and maintained at 85% of their free-feeding weights one week prior to and during experimentation. Water was available ad libitum. Rats were maintained on a 12-hour light/dark cycle. Apparatus All shaping, training, and testing took place in the rats’ home cages. Plastic Nalgene® cups (125 ml size) were used for the odor discriminations and were mounted using Velcro® onto rectangular Plexiglas® bases. Odor discriminations were created by mixing play sand (148.5 grams) with different dried spices (i.e., 1.5 grams of cocoa or cinnamon). Procedure Rats were shaped to dig in unscented cups of sand for three days prior to training (10 shaping trials total – see Figure 1 for shaping, training, and testing procedure). Rats received one day of training during which they learned one odor discrimination. One day following training, rats received at least two extinction trials for the odor discrimination with a 1 mg/kg cycloheximide injection (n=7) or a vehicle injection (n=6). Twenty-four hours later, rats received two test trials. Discussion Rats learned to dig in the correct odor and showed a statistically significant preference for it in both groups during acquisition. Administration of a protein synthesis inhibitor (cycloheximide) during extinction did not block memory for extinction (as indicated by preference scores that did not differ from chance during testing). Results suggest that protein synthesis may not be critical for the new learning associated with extinction in an appetitive odor discrimination task. References Bunsey, M. & Eichenbaum, H. (1996). Conservation of hippocampal memory function in rats and humans. Nature, 379, Lattal, K. M. & Abel, T. (2001). Different requirements for protein synthesis in acquisition and extinction of spatial preferences and context- evoked fear. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21(15), Meiri, N. & Rosenblum, K. (1998). Lateral ventricle injection of the protein synthesis inhibitor anisomycin impairs long-term memory in a spatial memory task. Brain Research, 789, Nader, K., Schafe, G. E., & LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature, 406, Suzuki, A., Josselyn, S. A., Frankland, P. W., Masushige, S., Silva, A. J., & Kida, S. (2004). Memory reconsolidation and extinction have distinct temporal and biochemical signatures. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24(20),