Experiment 2 (N=10) Purpose: Examine the ability of rare abrupt onsets (20% of trials) to capture attention away from a relevant cue. Design: Half of the.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
All slides © S. J. Luck, except as indicated in the notes sections of individual slides Slides may be used for nonprofit educational purposes if this copyright.
Advertisements

ERP correlates of retrieval orientation: cue- related and item-related measures Jane E. Herron and Edward L. Wilding, School of Psychology, Cardiff University.
Electrophysiology of Visual Attention. Does Visual Attention Modulate Visual Evoked Potentials? The theory is that Visual Attention modulates visual information.
Stages of Selection Broadbent: Early Selection - a bottleneck exists early in the course of sensory processing that filters out all but the attended channel.
Results and Discussion Logan Pedersen & Dr. Mei-Ching Lien School of Psychological Science, College of Liberal Arts Introduction A classic finding in Psychology.
P3 target cue target long CTI cue target cue short CTI children old PZ cue target Cue-related ERPs.
The Physiology of Attention. Physiology of Attention Neural systems involved in orienting Neural correlates of selection.
Attention-Dependent Hemifield Asymmetries When Judging Numerosity Nestor Matthews & Sarah Theobald Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville.
Attentionally Dependent Bilateral Advantage on Numerosity Judgments Jenny Ewing & Nestor Matthews Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville.
Control of Attention Major Distinctions: VoluntaryReflexive Overt Covert or.
Visual Attention Attention is the ability to select objects of interest from the surrounding environment A reliable measure of attention is eye movement.
Visual Attention: Outline Levels of analysis 1.Subjective: perception of unattended things 2.Functional: tasks to study components of attention 3.Neurological:
NEUR 3680 Midterm II Review Megan Metzler
Psych 216: Movement Attention. What is attention? There is too much information available in the world to process it all. Demonstration: change-detection.
Spatial Neglect and Attention Networks
Read this article for next week: A Neural Basis for Visual Search in Inferior Temporal Cortex Leonardo Chelazzi et al. (1993) Nature.
 The results of Experiment 2 replicated those of Experiment 1. Error rates were comparable for younger adults (2.4%) and older adults (2.1%).  Again,
Participants seem to utilize predictive information to actively maintain both task sets when doing so aids performance. Decreased mixing and switch costs.
Take your test today by 5!. Shadowing Many early studies employed variations on a paradigm called “shadowing” “Four score and seven years ago…” “It was.
Lab 9&10: Attention and Inhibition of Return
Office Hours Today are Relocated to CCBN rm EP1216 (the receptionist can help you find me)
INTRODUCTION Emotional stimuli that are irrelevant to task performance may redirect attentional resources, particularly among individuals evidencing high.
Read this article for Wednesday: A Neural Basis for Visual Search in Inferior Temporal Cortex Leonardo Chelazzi et al. (1993) Nature.
The Event-Related Potential (ERP) Embedded in the EEG signal is the small electrical response due to specific events such as stimulus or task onsets, motor.
Orienting Attention. Control of Attention Major Distinctions: VoluntaryReflexive.
Electrophysiology of Visual Attention. Does Visual Attention Modulate Visual Evoked Potentials? The theory is that Visual Attention modulates visual information.
Read article by Anne Treisman. Stages of Selection.
Disorders of Orienting Lesions to parietal cortex can produce some strange behavioural consequences –patients fail to notice events on the contralesional.
Abstract Cognitive control processes reduce the effects of irrelevant or misleading information on performance. We report a study suggesting that effective.
Orienting Attention Posner Cue - Target Paradigm: Subject presses a button as soon as x appears.
Uncertainty, Neuromodulation and Attention Angela Yu, and Peter Dayan.
Consequences of Attentional Selection Single unit recordings.
Home language environment and brain functions for language Irina Kuzmina, Amanda Hampton Wray, Eric Pakulak, Yoshiko Yamada, & Helen Neville Brain Development.
Block Types: Pure blocks of singleton search or feature search, plus mixed blocks of singleton search and feature search. Predictions Singleton Search:
Change blindness and time to consciousness Professor: Liu Student: Ruby.
Adaptive, behaviorally gated, persistent encoding of task-relevant auditory information in ferret frontal cortex.
Stimulus Visibility Reflected in Microsaccade Activity Jie Cui 1, Melanie Wilke 2, Nikos Logothetis 3, David Leopold 2, Hualou Liang 1 1 School of Health.
Attention Part 2. Early Selection Model (Broadbent, 1958) inputdetectionrecognition FI L T E R Only information that passed the filter received further.
Methods Inhibition of Return was used as a marker of attention capture.  After attention goes to a location it is inhibited from returning later. Results.
Localization of Auditory Stimulus in the Presence of an Auditory Cue By Albert Ler.
Training Phase Results The RT difference between gain and loss was numerically larger for the second half of the trials than the first half, as predicted,
Psych 435 Attention. Issues Capacity –We can’t respond to everything in the environment –Too many pieces of information –we can only actively respond.
Does the GNAT incongruent trial make cognitive conflict? Brain activities during Go/No-Go Association Task Koichi Hioki 1, Seiji Nakagawa 2, Hiroshi Watanabe.
Task Design EX: Participants were required to fixate on a plus sign in the middle of a screen. Sentences missing the last word were presented auditorily.
As expected, a large N400 effect was observed for all 3 word types in both experiments, |ts|≥7.69, ps
Sequence of Events in Spatial Cueing Paradigm +. +  time.
Results Introduction Nonconditional Feedback Selectively Eliminates Conflict Adaption Summary Methods 38 participants performed a parity judgment task.
The effects of working memory load on negative priming in an N-back task Ewald Neumann Brain-Inspired Cognitive Systems (BICS) July, 2010.
Interference from irrelevant color-singletons during serial search depends on visual attention being spatially diffuse Bryan R. Burnham James H. Neely.
Pattern Classification of Attentional Control States S. G. Robison, D. N. Osherson, K. A. Norman, & J. D. Cohen Dept. of Psychology, Princeton University,
Introduction Can you read the following paragraph? Can we derive meaning from words even if they are distorted by intermixing words with numbers? Perea,
Effects of the Hippopotamus on Explicit and Implicit Memory.
Psych 335 Attention. Issues Capacity –We can’t respond to everything in the environment –Too many pieces of information –we can only actively respond.
Orienting Attention to Semantic Categories T Cristescu, JT Devlin, AC Nobre Dept. Experimental Psychology and FMRIB Centre, University of Oxford, Oxford,
The role of working memory in eye-gaze cueing Anna S. Law, Liverpool John Moores University Stephen R. H. Langton, University of Stirling Introduction.
Video Games and Working Memory Derek M. Ellis Chris Blais Gene A. Brewer Department of Psychology Arizona State University The Entertainment Software Rating.
Effect of laterality-specific training on visual learning Jenna Kelly & Nestor Matthews Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville OH
ANT Z=52 R ACUE - PASSIVE VCUE - PASSIVE 1300 msVoltageCSD.31uV.03uV/cm 2 AIM We investigate the mechanisms of this hypothesized switch-ERP.
Laterality-Specific Perceptual Learning on Gabor Detection Nestor Matthews & Jenna Kelly Department of Psychology, Denison University, Granville OH
Without Words for Emotions: Is the emotional processing deficit in alexithymia caused by dissociation or suppression? Christian Sinnott & Dr. Mei-Ching.
The Effect of Retro-Cueing on an ERP Marker of VSTM Maintenance Alexandra M Murray, Bo-Cheng Kuo, Mark G Stokes, Anna C Nobre Brain & Cognition Laboratory,
Melanie Boysen & Gwendolyn Walton
Journal of Vision. 2011;11(10):16. doi: / Figure Legend:
Martin Eimer, Anna Grubert  Current Biology 
Direct Two-Dimensional Access to the Spatial Location of Covert Attention in Macaque Prefrontal Cortex  Elaine Astrand, Claire Wardak, Pierre Baraduc,
Stephen V. David, Benjamin Y. Hayden, James A. Mazer, Jack L. Gallant 
The Normalization Model of Attention
John T. Serences, Geoffrey M. Boynton  Neuron 
Beth L. Parkin, Hamed Ekhtiari, Vincent F. Walsh  Neuron 
Judging Peripheral Change: Attentional and Stimulus-Driven Effects
Presentation transcript:

Experiment 2 (N=10) Purpose: Examine the ability of rare abrupt onsets (20% of trials) to capture attention away from a relevant cue. Design: Half of the participants responded to the red letter in the target display and the other half responded to the green letter. The cue display contained a box colored the same as the target letter, which would normally capture attention. On 80% of the trials, this relevant cue appeared by itself, and on the other 20% of trials, it had to compete with a simultaneous abrupt onset (the relevant+onset cue). The onset appeared in the same hemifield as the relevant cue (but not the same location) for 10% of the trials, but in the other hemifield for the other 10% of the trials (see below for examples for the target color red). Results and Discussion Behavioral Data: N2pc Effects: The relevant cue by itself elicited a substantial N2pc effect ( µV), ts(9)≥- 2.19, ps≤.05. When competing with an abrupt onset, the relevant cue still elicited a strong N2pc effect ( µV; ts(9)≥-1.33, ps ≤.22); the difference was not significant, F<1.0. Conclusions Using behavioral and electrophysiological measures, we found no evidence that salient-but-irrelevant objects (color singletons and abrupt onsets) have the power to capture spatial attention, even when presented rarely (20% of trials). We found that capture was driven primarily by top-down control settings, not bottom-up stimulus salience. Introduction Can salient objects capture our attention? Some have argued that the salient stimuli, such as color singletons, do not have inherent power to capture our spatial attention unless they contain some properties match those we are currently looking for (contingent capture; e.g., Folk, Remington, & Johnston, 1992; Lien, Ruthruff, Goodin, & Remington, 2008). Others have argued that frequent present presentation of the salient objects diminished their power to capture attention (e.g., Horstmann & Ansorge, 2006). The present study examined capture by salient objects when presented only occasionally, using both behavioral measures (response time [RT] and proportion of error [PE]) and electrophysiological measures. The Present Study The goal of the present study was to determine whether salient-but-irrelevant objects can trigger the involuntary capture when presented rarely. We used a cuing paradigm, with non-informative cues (25% valid vs. 75% invalid). To assess capture, we measured cue validity effects (e.g., shorter RT for valid trials than invalid trials). We also assessed capture using an electrophysiological measure - the N2pc - believed to reflect the allocation of spatial attention. When attention is directed to the left or right visual field, brain potentials are found to be more negative in the contralateral hemisphere than in the ipsilateral hemisphere. The N2pc is the difference in event-related brain potentials (ERPs) between these hemispheres. It is usually strongest over posterior scalp, about ms after stimulus onset. The N2pc effect provides an index of whether and when attention has been directed to the cue. N2pc Effect = ERP contralateral to cue– ERP Ipsilateral to cue Experiment 1 (N = 16) Purpose: Determine whether an irrelevant color singleton has the power to capture spatial attention, producing a cuing validity effect and an N2pc, when it appears rarely. Target Display: Each participant responded to either the red letter, the green letter, the blue letter, or the yellow letter (4 groups). The target displays contained two T’s and two L’s. For the red and green target groups, one letter was red, one was green, and two were white. For the blue and yellow target groups, one letter was blue, one was yellow, and two were white. The two colored letters always appeared on opposite sides of the display. Cue Display: 80% of the trials (1024 trials in total) contained no cue, whereas 20% of the trials (256 trials in total) contained an irrelevant color singleton. For the red and green target groups, the color singleton cue was a blue box (among yellow boxes) or a yellow box (among blue boxes). For the blue and yellow target groups, the color singleton cue was a red box (among green boxes) or a green box (among red boxes). Sample Event Sequence: The target was red in this example. EEG Data Analyses: EEG epochs were time-locked to singleton cue onset. The cue-elicited N2pc was assessed ms after cue onset; the target- elicited N2pc was assessed ms after cue onset. Sites: O1, O2, PO5, PO6, P5, and P6 Predictions: If a salient-but-irrelevant singleton can capture attention when presented rarely, the cue should produce cue validity effects and N2pc effects. Results and Discussion Behavioral Data: N2pc Effects (cue present trials): The irrelevant color singleton cue produced no N2pc effect during the period ms after cue onset, ts(15)<1.0, suggesting that it did not capture attention, despite being presented rarely. Electrophysiological Evidence for the Failure of Salient Stimuli to Capture Attention, even when Presented Rarely Birken Noesen & Mei-Ching Lien Eric Ruthruff Oregon State University University of New Mexico References Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, Horstmann, G., & Ansorge, U. (2006). Attentional shifts to rare singletons. Visual Cognition, 14, Lien, M.-C., Ruthruff, E., Goodin, Z., & Remington, R. W. (2008). Contingent attentional capture by top-down control settings: Converging evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, Mean RT was not significantly different between the no-cue condition and the valid and invalid cue conditions, Fs(1,12)≤1.78, ps≥.2072, indicating that the salient- but-irrelevant cue did not capture attention. OR Cue displays for Red/Green targets OR Cue displays for Blue/Yellow targets T T L L FixationCue Tone Feedback 1200 ms Target 100 ms ms50 ms100 ms50 msUntil Response100 ms TIME 527 ms533 ms Cue and target in the same hemifield Cue and target in different hemifields Pooled Cue-Elicited N2pc Effects Target-Elicited N2pc Effects * p < ms*68 ms* Valid Invalid 10% Same Hemifield 80% Relevant + Onset Cue 10% Different Hemifields Relevant Cue Only Relevant cues (in the target color) produced large cue validity effects, regardless of whether there was a simultaneous abrupt onset, ts(9)≥7.68, ps< Instead of reducing capture by the relevant stimulus, the onset may have actually increased capture, F(1,8)=3.50, p=.09. Cue and target in the same hemifield Cue and target in different hemifields Relevant Cue Relevant + Onset Cue Target-Elicited N2pc Effects Cue-Elicited N2pc Effects Acknowledgements This research was supported by funding from Oregon State University Undergraduate Research, Innovation, Scholarship, and Creativity to Birken Noesen.