Selecting Instructional Principles Ken Koedinger Reading: KLI paper sections 6-7 1.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
How Students Learn Mathematics in the Classroom June 18, 2009.
Advertisements

ESP410 Human Movement Pedagogy 3
The Computer as a Tutor. With the invention of the microcomputer (now also commonly referred to as PCs or personal computers), the PC has become the tool.
Learning Outcomes Participants will be able to analyze assessments
How Do People Learn From e-Courses? Chapter 2 Ken Koedinger Based on slides from Ruth Clark 1.
Advances in the PARCC Mathematics Assessment August
1 LearnLab: Bridging the Gap Between Learning Science and Educational Practice Ken Koedinger Human-Computer Interaction & Psychology, CMU PI & CMU Director.
Thinking ‘Behind’ the Steps Engaging Students in Thinking ‘Behind’ the Steps.
Careers in Education Teaching Strategies. Learning Target: Students will be able to… – Explain in words the 3 different teaching strategies – Apply the.
An Overview of Webb’s Depth of Knowledge
 Metacognition refers to a learner’s ability to be aware of and monitor their own learning processes.  Usually defined by it’s component parts.
Knowledge Acquisitioning. Definition The transfer and transformation of potential problem solving expertise from some knowledge source to a program.
An Introduction to Online Teaching and Learning
Video Cases Online: Cognitive Studies of Pre- Service Teacher Learning Sharon J. Derry University of Wisconsin-Madison Cindy Hmelo-Silver Rutgers University.
Teaching and Learning If you don’t know anything about learning… You don’t know anything about teaching! Telling is not TEACHING Listening is not LEARNING.
Teaching with Depth An Understanding of Webb’s Depth of Knowledge
Principles of High Quality Assessment
Meaningful Learning in an Information Age
Writing Instructional Objectives
Cognitive Science Overview Design Activity Cognitive Apprenticeship Theory Cognitive Flexibility Theory.
LANGUAGE LEARNING STRATEGIES
46th Annual MPESA Fall Conference
Teaching with Depth An Understanding of Webb’s Depth of Knowledge
Complex Cognitive Processes
CURRICULUM EVALUATION. Citation and Skill Focus  Charles, R. I., et al. (1999). Math, Teacher’s Edition, Vol 2. New York: Scott Foresman-Addison Wesley.
Click to edit Master title style  Click to edit Master text styles  Second level  Third level  Fourth level  Fifth level  Click to edit Master text.
Robust Learning in Culturally Relevant Algebra Problem Scenarios Candace DiBiano Department of Science and Math Education University of Texas at Austin.
Instructional design principles and models Mart Laanpere, M.Sc. in educational technology Tallinn University of Educational and Social Sciences
The Cognitive Load Theory
A Framework for Inquiry-Based Instruction through
DOK Depth of Knowledge An Introduction.
Advances in the PARCC Mathematics Assessment August
Webb’s Depth of Knowledge
C Pearson Allyn & Bacon Encoding and Retrieval Processes in Long-Term Memory Chapter 6.
Building Interactivity into MultiMedia: Theory into Practice by Lauren Cifuentes.
Applying the Multimedia Principle: Use Words and Graphics Rather than Words Alone Chapter 4 Ken Koedinger 1.
T 7.0 Chapter 7: Questioning for Inquiry Chapter 7: Questioning for Inquiry Central concepts:  Questioning stimulates and guides inquiry  Teachers use.
Rational/Theoretical Cognitive Task Analysis Ken Koedinger Key reading: Zhu, X., & Simon, H. A. (1987). Learning mathematics from examples and by doing.
Academic Needs of L2/Bilingual Learners
Evidence-based Practice Chapter 3 Ken Koedinger Based on slides from Ruth Clark 1.
Dick Clark, Rich DiNinni and Gary Rauchfuss November , 2006
How People Learn – Brain, Mind, Experience, and School (Bransford, Brown, & Cocking, 1999) Three core principles 1: If their (students) initial understanding.
Chapter 3 Human Resource Development
JOT2 – LEARNING THEORIES
Applying the Guidelines ( Chapter 17) Ken Koedinger 1.
Does Practice Make Perfect? ( Chapter 12) Ken Koedinger 1.
KLI & selecting appropriate instructional principles Ken Koedinger 1.
How people learn different ways to think about learning.
Promoting Deep Learning “A person with a brain full of knowledge is not a teacher … until he or she can convey that knowledge to another person.”
RULES Patty Nordstrom Hien Nguyen. "Cognitive Skills are Realized by Production Rules"
Teaching with Depth An Understanding of Webb’s Depth of Knowledge.
GENERAL METHODS AND TECHNIQUES OF TEACHING
February, 2013 Jocelyn Paul Deployment Manager Microsoft Learning Microsoft IT Academy Program.
Structuring Learning. Agenda Structuring learning. Structuring lab sessions. Engagement. Critical Thinking. Ideas for structuring learning. Activity.
ELED 6560 Summer Learning Exercises #10 The Un-Natural Part of Teaching  Five Ways that Teaching Behavior is Un-Natural 1. Helping Others 2.
Second Language Acquisition and Theory Julie Lucas
Instructional Strategies
Teaching with Depth An Understanding of Webb’s Depth of Knowledge
Using Cognitive Science To Inform Instructional Design
ED 260-Educational Psychology
OSEP Leadership Conference July 28, 2015 Margaret Heritage, WestEd
LECTURE 4. LEARNING AND TEACHING PROCESSES
Does Learning from Examples Improve Tutored Problem Solving?
Fostering Higher-Order Learning in STEM Education: A Role for Science of Learning Victor Benassi Faculty Director, Center for Excellence and Innovation.
Principles of teaching and learning and teaching methods
The curriculum The curricullum tells «What and how the learners should learn» at specific levels of the education system. It includes the objectives and.
GENERAL METHODS AND TECHNIQUES OF TEACHING
Vincent Aleven & Kirsten Butcher
Julie Booth, Robert Siegler, Ken Koedinger & Bethany Rittle-Johnson
Guided Math.
Presentation transcript:

Selecting Instructional Principles Ken Koedinger Reading: KLI paper sections 6-7 1

Key points Learning is mostly subconscious/tacit/implicit – Be skeptical of your own intuitions & conscious reflections/beliefs about learning & instruction Human learning is substantially influenced by existing knowledge – What you already know changes how & how well you learn => subject-matter/domain analysis is crucial Optimal instruction is knowledge-dependent, not domain-general – Need cognitive models of domains (from CTA) to guide selection & application of instructional principles

Cognitive Psychology: Implicit knowledge examples What you know Declarative Structure of perceived world/objects Linguistic memories: Words, definitions, verbal rules Procedural Conditions & logic behind decisions, planning Physical actions, verbal expressions Working memory Visual contents of WM Linguistic contents of WM L1 grammar, face recognition, chess board, deep features Perceptual chunks: expand experts’ memory, organize “intuitive” reasoning & evaluation,

Cognitive Psychology: Implicit knowledge examples What you know Declarative Structure of perceived world/objects Linguistic memories: Words, definitions, verbal rules Procedural Conditions & logic behind decisions, planning Physical actions, verbal expressions Working memory Visual contents of WM Linguistic contents of WM Choices in surgery, planning a proof, social/design decisions Amnesic “HM” learns tower of Hanoi but no declarative k.

How much of each? What you know Declarative Structure of perceived world/objects Linguistic memories: Words, definitions, verbal rules Procedural Conditions & logic behind decisions, planning Physical actions, verbal expressions Working memory Visual contents of WM Linguistic contents of WM What you don’t know you know ~ 70% What you know you know ~ 30%

Experts don’t know what they know Example think aloud from Arka’s project: “ok I am recalling[1]…um ok first fold it in half first[2]….umm[3]…ok I got it, I think I got it[4]….so at this point it’s kind of routine but the real point where I start to think …ok I have to remember this is at this point…ok so the top left side is going down[5]…so the rest of my models have to be this way…..so I believe you tuck this in….and then just…and then at this part just…..folding it in…oh gosh![6]...am I right ?...folding it in..this part is just kinda[7]…tucking it in..there you go…and..we are done 6

KLI Blank slate learning and Instructional events Explanation, practice, text, rule, example, teacher-student discussion Assessment events Question, feedback, step in ITS Learning events Knowledge Components KEY Ovals – observable Rectangles - inferred Arrows – causal links Exam, belief survey Koedinger et al. (2012). The Knowledge-Learning-Instruction (KLI) framework: Bridging the science-practice chasm to enhance robust student learning. Cognitive Science. KLI: Blank slate learning + knowledge-based learning

Cognitive Tutor Principles Problem solving context (testing) Minimize working memory Base instruction on a model of student skills & concepts Domain-specific Domain-general

>300 LearnLab in vivo experiments: Correlation between complexity of knowledge & complexity of instruction that best produces it Koedinger et al. (2012). The Knowledge-Learning- Instruction (KLI) framework: Bridging the science- practice chasm to enhance robust student learning. Cognitive Science.

KLI Dependency Optimal Instructional choices depend on which of many possible Learning processes are needed to achieve which of many possible Knowledge acquisition goals “the size of the spacing effect declined sharply as conceptual difficulty of the task increased” (Donovan & Radosevich, 1999) “situations with low processing demands benefit from practice conditions that increase the load … whereas practice conditions that result in extremely high load should benefit from conditions that reduce the load” ( Wulf & Shea, 2002) Koedinger et al. (2012). The Knowledge-Learning-Instruction (KLI) framework: Bridging the science-practice chasm to enhance robust student learning. Cognitive Science.

KLI: More complex learning processes are effective for more complex knowledge

Desirable difficulties vs. cognitive load theory/direct instruction Desirable difficulties (Bjork et al) – Spacing, delayed feedback, testing effect – “Tests enhance later retention more than additional study of the material” (Roediger & Karpicke) Cognitive load theory & direct instruction (Sweller, Mayer, Klahr) – Multimedia principles, worked examples – “a worked example constitutes the epitome of strongly guided instruction” (Kirschner, Sweller, Clark)

14 How are testing effect & worked examples paradigms similar? Instructors AssistanceHighLow Students Difficulty/LoadLowHigh GiveElicit StudyDo Testing Effect Study lao3shi1 -> teacher Test lao3shi1 -> ? Worked Examples Example Solve (a+b)/c = d -> (a+b)/c = d a+b = dc a = dc - b Problem Solve (a+b)/c = d -> ?

15 Study & example give, test & problem elicit StudyDo Testing Effect Study A -> B Test A -> ? Worked Examples Example A -> B Problem A -> ? Students Instructors AssistanceHighLow Difficulty/LoadLowHigh GiveElicit

16 Results are contradictory! StudyDo Testing Effect Study (SSST) 57% Test (STTT) 64% Worked Examples Examples (EP EP EP EP) 88% Problems (PP PP PP PP) 42% Students Instructors AssistanceHighLow Difficulty/LoadLowHigh GiveElicit Roediger & Karpick, 06 Sweller & Cooper, 85

KLI helps explain discrepancy Different knowledge goals => different learning processes => different instruction is optimal KnowledgeLearningInstruction Testing effect Constant- constant facts MemoryEliciting is better Worked examples Variable- variable rules InductionMore giving is better

Target Knowledge => Learning processes => Instruction Worked examples Testing effect Eliciting recall supports Aids fact learning, but suboptimal for rules Many examples support Aid rule learning, but suboptimal for facts

Worked Examples Specific to KCs See example from Fraction Numberline game 19

END 20

Can interactive tutoring of rule KCs be improved by adding examples? No by desirable difficulties/testing effect – Eliciting is better when it works – Feedback provides examples when it doesn’t Yes by cognitive load theory/worked examples – Examples support induction & deeper feature search – Early problems introduce load => shallow processing & less attention to example-based feedback Test with lab & in vivo experiments …

Ecological Control = Standard Cognitive Tutor Students solve problems step- by-step & explain

Worked out steps with calculation shown by Tutor Treatments: 1) Half of steps are given as examples 2) Adaptive fading of examples into problems Student still has to self explain worked out step

d =.73 * Lab results: Adding examples yields better conceptual transfer & 20% less instructional time

Results: In Vivo study  Result is robust in classroom environment: adaptive fading examples > problem solving

Comments on discussion board posts from 2013 Worked examples should be focused on solving real-world-like tasks that promote learning transfer. – Interesting claim, which has merit, but for which the empirical evidence is mixed, with a number of results that better transfer is achieved with more abstract, less real-world examples (and tasks) We should avoid creating "regurgitated" work examples that are just repetitions of the instructional or practice tasks presented in the manner of a worked example. Fading worked examples into exercises is generally good practice. For example, instead of having the worked example for an Excel lesson be gradually filling out sentences like: "Cells are spaces where ______, formulas are ________", a real- world calculation scenario should be presented: ” – Are we clear on what a worked example is and how it is different from a problem? Mary needs to find out how much she has earned from the book she has sold using Excel. We first fill out the names of the books and the initial inventory...". Worked examples should never exceed practice exercises. Usually, two worked examples per type of problems suffice in subjects like mathematics. After this, they can be made to gradually fade out to practice exercises. – Not “per type of problem” but “per knowledge component” 26