Reciprocity between Humor and Peer Victimization

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Method Participants 184 five-year-old (M age=5.63, SD=0.22) kindergarten students from 30 classrooms in central Illinois Teacher ratings The second edition.
Advertisements

The Humour and bullying project: Modelling cross-lagged and dyadic data. Dr Simon C. Hunter School of Psychological Sciences and Health, University of.
Maternal Psychological Control: Links to Close Friendship and Depression in Early Adolescence Heather L. Tencer Jessica R. Meyer Felicia D. Hall University.
Bullying, Academic Performance, and High School Dropout in Adolescence Victoria Hirsch 1, Meredith Henry, Sylvie Mrug 1 and Michael Windle 2 1 University.
Adolescent Attachment to Parents: Predicting Later Adolescent Rejection Sensitivity I would like to thank the William T. Grant Foundation, Spencer Foundation,
Both self-esteem and co-rumination have been shown to influence an individual’s psychological well-being. Rose (2002) defined co- rumination as “excessively.
Abstract Investigated the mother-child relationship and adolescents’ emotional symptoms and adaptive skills reported by youth. Negative mother-child relationship.
The development of a humour styles questionnaire for younger children Background and Rationale Fox et al. (2011) developed a reliable and valid scale to.
O Does humor impact relationship satisfaction? o Four different styles of humor (Martin, Puhlik-Doris, Larsen, Gray, & Weir, 2003)  Humor which enhances/protects.
Introduction In adult relationships, humor use is a highly desirable characteristic in potential friends and romantic partners (Sprecher & Regan, 2002)
Dysfunctional Individuation Mediates the Relationship between Attachment Styles and Disordered Eating Erin E. Reilly, Paul C. Stey, & Daniel Lapsley Available.
What Makes the Finger Point Internally? Predictors of Self-Blame/Guilt in Sexually Abused Boys and Girls ????? ???????? University of Nebraska-Lincoln.
By Claire Fox 1, Simon Hunter 2, Lucy James 1 and Hayley Gilman 1 1 Keele University, 2 University of Strathclyde Box 1: Humour Styles Adaptive: Affiliative.
BACKGROUND Evolutionary Personality Psychology is the study of individual differences in evolved systems for social behavior. The most familiar and best.
Pubertal Timing and Tempo: Associations with Friends’ Behavior in Early and Middle Adolescence Catherine Thompson 1, Meredith Henry 2, Sylvie Mrug 2 1.
The role of humor in human relationships: An evolutionary model
I Think I’m OK, Why Don’t You?: The Saga of Disagreeable Youth Christopher A. Hafen, Megan M. Schad, Elendra T. Hessel, Joseph P. Allen University of Virginia.
This research was funded by the Economic and Social Research Council, award reference RES Humour styles as moderators and mediators of the.
Relational Vulnerability: A Model for Understanding Girls, Aggression, and Adjustment Nicki Crick & Tasha Geiger University of Minnesota USA Melanie Zimmer-Gembeck.
Self Competence and Depressive Symptoms in Ethnic Minority Students: The Role of Ethnic Identity and School Belonging Praveena Gummadam and Laura D. Pittman.
Social and Emotional Costs of “One-Sided Friendships” in Adolescence. Lauren Molloy & Joseph P. Allen University of Virginia We would like to thank the.
Participants & Procedures  and 19-year-old college students were recruited from an introductory psychology course at a large Midwestern University.
Friends (Temporarily) Forever: Frequency of Facebook Use, Relationship Satisfaction, and Perception of Friendship Zack Hayes, Jerad Hill, Heather Jacobson,
Experiences with Parents, Peers, and Romantic Partners During Adolescence as Predictors of Youths’ Emotion Regulation Strategies David E. Szwedo, M.A.
High Narcissism and Low Self-Esteem as Risk Factors for the Development of Conduct Problems and Aggression in Children Kristy K. Adler 1, Christopher T.
The Broader Context of Relational Aggression in Adolescent Romantic Relationships Megan M. Schad, David E. Szwedo, Amanda Hare, Jill Antonishak, Joseph.
Table 2: Correlation between age and readiness to change Table 1: T-test relating gender and readiness to change  It is estimated that 25% of children.
Expecting the worst often leads to poor outcomes. This process is particularly true in close relationships, as those who are most sensitive to rejection.
Introduction Early Childhood Characteristics and Academic Achievement  Research has demonstrated continuity between toddler- age language ability and.
Dr. Siân Jones, #PITPfriends wordpress.com FRIENDS LIKE THESE: PEER.
Introduction Disordered eating continues to be a significant health concern for college women. Recent research shows it is on the rise among men. Media.
Early Adolescent Behaviors in Disagreement with Best Friend Predictive of Later Emotional Repair Abilities Lauren Cannavo, Elenda T. Hessel, Joseph S.
Temperament Constitutionally based individual differences in behavioral characteristics that are relatively consistent across situations and over time.
Results Baseline Differences Between Groups No significant differences were found between ethnic groups on baseline levels of Praise (F = 2.006, p>.05),
Personal Control over Development: Effects on the Perception and Emotional Evaluation of Personal Development in Adulthood.
LMAO? Longitudinal relationships between humour and involvement in bullying. Dr Simon C. Hunter School of Psychological Sciences and Health, University.
Reactions to Humorous Comments and Implicit Theories of Humor Styles Nicholas A. Kuiper, Gillian A. Kirsh, and Catherine Leite Presented by Willie Hallford.
Risky driving  Patterns of driving behavior that place drivers at risk for mortality,  Involve legal violations  Do NOT involve alcohol or drug use.
The emergence of depressive symptoms from late childhood into adolescence in the ALSPAC cohort: impact of age, gender and puberty Carol Joinson, Jon Heron.
BULLYING AND BELONGING: A LONGITUDINAL STUDY OF THE PROTECTIVE ROLE OF DEFENDERS IN FRIENDSHIP GROUPS Siân Jones, Claire Fox, Simon Hunter, & Jon Kennedy.
◦ th and 11 th grade high school students (54% girls) ◦ 63% Caucasian; 24% African-American; 13% Hispanic; remaining were Asian or “other” ◦ Mean.
General and Feeding Specific Behavior Problems in a Community Sample of Children Amy J. Majewski, Kathryn S. Holman & W. Hobart Davies University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee.
BULLYING AND BELONGING: A LONGITUDINAL STUDY OF THE PROTECTIVE ROLE OF DEFENDERS IN FRIENDSHIP GROUPS Siân Jones, Claire Fox, Simon Hunter, & Jon Kennedy.
Selfishness and Self-Mattering: Do I Fit In? Leah Burke Advisors: Ellen Cohn, Ph.D. & Alexander Blandina, M.A. The University of New Hampshire INTRODUCTION.
Positive Discipline Parenting Workshops Increase Confidence in Parents, Increase Academic Competence in Children BACKGROUND Positive Discipline is a type.
Mankind’s Greatest Blessing: Personal Humor Style and Humor Appreciation By: Steven LaCorte The purpose of this study was to examine the relationship between.
Observations Adult report. Child reports Sociograms- these plot the relationships between people.
Romantic Partners Promotion of Autonomy and Relatedness in Adolescence as a Predictor of Young Adult Emotion Regulation. Elenda T. Hessel, Emily L. Loeb,
Florida International University, Miami, FL
An evaluation of the online universal COPING parent programme:
Christian Hahn, M.Sc. & Lorne Campbell, PhD
Introduction Hypotheses Results Discussion Method
Laughing Together Or Joking Apart?
Introduction Results Hypotheses Discussion Method
Introduction Results Hypotheses Discussion Method
The Role of Adolescent Relationships in Predicting Withdrawal in Emerging Adulthood J. Claire Stephenson, Amanda L. Hare, Nell N. Manning & Joseph P.
Introduction Discussion Results Method References
Introduction Results Methods Conclusions
Testing the Attachment Theory of Parent-Child Interaction Therapy Erin Floyd and Sheila Eyberg Department of Clinical and Health Psychology University.
Attachment Dependency
2University of Virginia
Introduction Results Hypotheses Discussion Method
Laura M. Sylke & David E. Szwedo James Madison University Introduction
Emily A. Davis & David E. Szwedo James Madison University Introduction
Maddison Miles & David E. Szwedo James Madison University
General Social Competence (18)
Attachment, Coping, & Social Competence
Korey F. Beckwith & David E. Szwedo James Madison University
The Effects of Childhood Emotional Abuse on Later Romantic Relationship Outcomes: The Moderating Role of Self-Worth, Alcohol, and Jealousy Madeline M.
Kristin E. Gross & David E. Szwedo James Madison University
Presentation transcript:

Reciprocity between Humor and Peer Victimization Dr Claire Fox1, Dr Simon Hunter2, Dr Siân Jones3 1Keele University, 2University of Strathclyde, 3Oxford Brookes University Contact: c.fox@keele.ac.uk Today I am going to present findings from the first stage of the ESRC funded Humour Styles and Bullying in Schools project, which I am working on with Sian Jones and Simon Hunter, who is based at the University of Strathclyde.

Humor Styles Questionnaire (Adult) Four dimensions: Self-enhancing (e.g. ‘My humorous outlook on life keeps me from getting too upset or depressed about things’) Aggressive (e.g. ‘If someone makes a mistake I often tease them about it’) Affiliative (e.g. ‘I enjoy making people laugh’) Self-defeating (e.g. ‘I often try to make people like me or accept me more by saying something funny about my own weaknesses, blunders or faults’) Data supports the reliability and validity of the HSQ (Martin et al, 2003; Martin, 2007) Much stronger correlations between humor styles and psychological adjustment….. Research using the Humour Styles Questionnaire for adults (developed by Martin et al, 2003) suggests that individual mental health may depend on how people use humour in their everyday lives. The HSQ is a theoretically driven measure which assumes that humour can be both adaptive (‘self-enhancing’ and ‘affiliative’) and maladaptive (‘aggressive’ and ‘self-defeating’). Self-enhancing humour is used to enhance the self, but is not detrimental to others (e.g. ‘My humorous outlook on life keeps me from getting too upset or depressed about things’). Aggressive humour on the other hand, while enhancing the self, at least in the short-term, is done at the expense of others (e.g. ‘If someone makes a mistake I often tease them about it’). Over the long-term, this style is believed to be detrimental to the self because it tends to alienate others (Martin, 2007). Affiliative humour enhances one’s relationships with others and reduces interpersonal tensions (e.g. ‘I enjoy making people laugh’). Self-defeating humour also enhances one’s relationships with others, but at the expense of denigration of the self (e.g. ‘I often try to make people like or accept me more by saying something funny about my own weaknesses, blunders and faults’). Martin et al (2003) state that, over the long term, it is damaging for the individual since it involves denigration of the self and repression of one’s own emotional needs In using the HSQ, Martin et al found much stronger correlations between humour styles and psychological adjustment compared to previous studies. It was argued that this was because previous questionnaires did not distinguish between adaptive and maladaptive styles of humour. Data collected on a large number of participants, aged 14-87 years, and in various different countries, supports the proposed four-factor structure of the scale, with all four sub-scales showing good internal consistency. Research has consistently provided data in support of the construct validity of each scale and the discriminant validity of all four scales (Martin, 2007). Thus, I was confident that the HSQ for children and adolescents would prove to be equally valid and reliable.

Humor and psychosocial adjustment in adults Adaptive forms of humor negatively correlated with depression/anxiety and positively correlated with self-esteem and life satisfaction Self-defeating humor – opposite effects (Martin et al., 2003; Kuiper et al., 2004) Maladaptive forms of humor – negative impact on interactions with others (Kuiper et al., 2010; Ziegler-Hill et al., 2013)

Humor and psychosocial adjustment in children Links between humor and social competence (Masten, 1986; Sherman, 1988) Klein and Kuiper (2006): Children who are bullied at a disadvantage with respect to the development of humor competence Gravitate to self-defeating humor Self-defeating humor as a risk factor for victimisation

Interpersonal risk model Kochel et al. (2012) Interpersonal risk model PV Adjustment problems Symptoms driven model Adjustment problems PV Transactional model PV Adjustment problems

Reciprocity - Peer Victimization and Humor More self-defeating Peer victimization Less affiliative Less self-enhancing Previous research – self-defeating humour and affiliative humour associated with self-perceived social competence.

ESRC Humor and Bullying Study Short-term longitudinal design Participants (Time 1) N = 1234: Gender: 599 male and 620 female (15 missing) Age: 11-13 years, mean age = 11.68(SD= .64) Measures: Peer nomination inventory Peer nominations of 4 types of victimisation Self-report questionnaires: Child HSQ (Fox et al., 2013) Self-report victimisation questionnaire to measure 3 types (Owens et al., 2005) These two strands map onto the functions of humour I outlined at the beginning: the social and emotional functions of humour.

Results Peer victimization (SR and PN) positively correlated with SD humor and negatively correlated with Aff humor at T1 and T2 PN of peer victimization negatively correlated with self-enhancing humor at T1 and T2 Analytic approach: Cross-lagged measurement models Self-report of humor styles SR of peer victimization PN of peer victimization Combined cross-lagged measurement models SR of peer victimization and humor styles PN of peer victimization and humor styles

Table 1: Cross-lagged measurement models χ2 df χ2/df CFI RMSEA Model 1: SR peer victimization 2964.68*** 575 5.16 .87 .058 Model 2: Model 1 with constraints Model 3: PN of peer victimization Model 4: SR of humor styles Model 5: Model 4 with constraints 2112.18*** 68.11*** 2515.22*** 2350.82*** 563 15 1029 1023 3.75 4.54 2.44 2.30 .92 .99 .91 .047 .054 .034 .032 ***p < .001. SR = Self-report. PN = Peer-nomination

Table 2: Full cross-lagged models combining peer victimization and humor styles χ2 df χ2/df CFI RMSEA Model 6: SR peer victimization and humor 6924.25*** 3298 2.10 .91 .030 Model 7: PN peer victimization and humor 2927.54*** 1406 2.08 .93 ***p < .001. SR = Self-report. PN = Peer-nomination

  Figure 1: Schematic of structural model for self-reported peer victimization and humor styles. Only significant paths shown.

Figure 2: Schematic of structural model for peer nominated peer victimization and humor styles. Only significant paths shown

Summary of findings Evidence of a vicious cycle between peer victimization and the use of adaptive and maladaptive humor styles Peer victimization appears to increase future use of self-defeating humor and decrease the use of affiliative humor At the same time, greater use of self-defeating humor increases the risk of later peer victimization, while greater use of affiliative humor reduces the risk of later victimization Support for Klein and Kuiper’s (2006) predictions And experimental studies that have examined the impact of the four humor styles on others

Conclusions For many years, young people have been encouraged to use humor as a way of dealing with the bullies, most notably by ‘fogging’ However, our evidence suggests this can lead to negative outcomes, whereas taking an approach based on affiliative humor is more likely to lead to positive outcomes Is it possible to teach children to use the more adaptive styles of humor and discourage use of aggressive and SD humor?

Further information http://esrcbullyingandhumourproject.wordpress.com/ Twitter @Humour_Bullying Email: c.fox@keele.ac.uk

Acknowledgements ESRC Rod Martin Sirandou Saidy Khan and Hayley Gilman Lucy James and Katie Wright-Bevans Teachers, parents and children

Thank you for listening