Gender Differences in Buffering Stress Responses in Same-Sex Friend Dyads Sydney N. Pauling, Jenalee R. Doom, & Megan R. Gunnar Institute of Child Development,

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
ANALYSIS OF TIME SPENT AND Frequency of use of SOCIAL MEDIA BY School grade RESEARCH CONDUCTED AND SUBMITTED BY MARIST COLLEGE ASHGROVE WITH ASSISTANCE.
Advertisements

Maternal Psychological Control: Links to Close Friendship and Depression in Early Adolescence Heather L. Tencer Jessica R. Meyer Felicia D. Hall University.
The Self-Fulfilling Prophecy of Adolescent Social Expectations. Emily L. Loeb, Elenda T. Hessel, Megan M. Schad, & Joseph P. Allen University of Virginia.
Gender attitudes and adolescent functioning in the context of romantic relationships Joseph W. Dickson 1 Melinda S. Harper 2 Deborah P. Welsh 1 1 University.
What Makes the Finger Point Internally? Predictors of Self-Blame/Guilt in Sexually Abused Boys and Girls ????? ???????? University of Nebraska-Lincoln.
The Need for Attachment: Demonstrations through Teens’ Photos on MySpace and Facebook Introduction SOCIAL NETWORKING WEBSITES: - Allow users to share information.
Method Introduction Mixed groups ANOVAs were performed and then followed up using the LSD procedure (p =.05) to look at the relationship between gender,
Method Introduction Results Discussion Weekly Symptom Variability in Sexually Abused Youth Undergoing Treatment: The Needs and Responses of Youth with.
Physical Aggression and Self-Injury in Juvenile Delinquent Nikki J. Deaver University of Nebraska-Lincoln Methods Participants: Participants were 43 youths.
Effect of Staff Attitudes on Quality in Clinical Microbiology Services Ms. Julie Sims Laboratory Technical specialist Strengthening of Medical Laboratories.
Chapter 7 Correlational Research Gay, Mills, and Airasian
Increasing Positive Affect and Social Responsiveness in Children and Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders: The Adaptation of a Music-Based Intervention.
Pubertal Timing and Tempo: Associations with Friends’ Behavior in Early and Middle Adolescence Catherine Thompson 1, Meredith Henry 2, Sylvie Mrug 2 1.
1 Psychology 320: Gender Psychology Lecture Friendship: 1. Are there sex differences in friendship? (continued)
I Think I’m OK, Why Don’t You?: The Saga of Disagreeable Youth Christopher A. Hafen, Megan M. Schad, Elendra T. Hessel, Joseph P. Allen University of Virginia.
Context and the Relationship Between Social Anxiety and Urge to Drink Tracey A. Garcia & Lindsay S. Ham Florida International University Introduction 
High levels of anxiety negatively influence children’s academic performance (Mazzone, Ducci, Scoto, Passaniti, D'Arrigo, & Vitiello, 2007). Social anxiety.
The Watson Institute Research indicates that school aged children with autism spectrum disorders are at greater risk for being bullied (Attwood, 2006;
Growing Up In Ireland Research Conference The Education of 9-Year-Olds.
The authors would like to acknowledge the families at the Children’s Hospital of Wisconsin. This project was funded by the Research Growth Initiative from.
The authors would like to acknowledge the families at the Children’s Hospital of Wisconsin. This project was funded by the Greater Milwaukee Fund. For.
Ethnic Identity among Mexican American Adolescents: The Role of Maternal Cultural Values and Parenting Practices 1 Miriam M. Martinez, 1 Gustavo Carlo,
Romantic Partner Selection and Relationship Quality in Adolescence: Do Parent or Early Peer Relationships Matter More? Joanna M. Chango, David E. Szwedo,
CONCLUSION There was a significant main effect of dyadic gender composition on dyadic tie strength, such that same-gender dyads demonstrated stronger ties.
Can Peer Pressure Be A Good Thing? Megan M. Schad, Meredyth A. Evans, David E. Szwedo, Joseph P. Allen University of Virginia We would like to thank the.
Peer Groups: Structure Typically range in size from 3 to >10 children (average = 5-6) Mostly composed of same-sex peers During childhood, peer groups tend.
The Broader Context of Relational Aggression in Adolescent Romantic Relationships Megan M. Schad, David E. Szwedo, Amanda Hare, Jill Antonishak, Joseph.
Table 2: Correlation between age and readiness to change Table 1: T-test relating gender and readiness to change  It is estimated that 25% of children.
Expecting the worst often leads to poor outcomes. This process is particularly true in close relationships, as those who are most sensitive to rejection.
1 Effect of Communication Ability on Cardiovascular Reactivity to a Speech Task Susie Kola & Jane Walsh Department of Psychology National University of.
Printed by The Aftercare and School Observation System: Characteristics of out-of-home contexts and young children’s behavior problems.
Early Adolescent Behaviors in Disagreement with Best Friend Predictive of Later Emotional Repair Abilities Lauren Cannavo, Elenda T. Hessel, Joseph S.
Youth violence exposure, adolescent delinquency and anxiety, and the potential mediating role of sleep problems during middle childhood Chelsea M. Weaver.
Playground Settings and the Impact of Recess on Classroom Attention Christine Peterson, B.A., M.S.E. Psychology Department Human Development Center University.
Early Adolescence Social Withdrawal as a Predictor of Late Adolescence Autonomy and Relatedness with Romantic Partners. Elenda T. Hessel, Megan M. Schad,
Negative Psychosocial Outcomes of Engaging in Sexual Intercourse Before the Age of 16 Introduction Mary K. Higgins Mary K. Higgins,
 By preschool age, boys and girls show marked differences on a number of emotional, social, and behavioral outcomes (Ruble et al., 2006). Some gender.
 1,001 adolescent boys (47%) and girls (53%)  Fairly diverse: 58% Caucasian; 23% African American,12% Hispanic, 2% Asian, 5% Other  Age Range:
The Role of Close Family Relationships in Predicting Multisystemic Therapy Outcome: An Investigation of Sex Differences ABSTRACT BACKGROUND: Multisystemic.
Results Time 2 (Age 18-20) Target teen and their romantic partner engaged in an 8 minute hypothetical disagreement task interaction. Hostile, relationship-undermining.
Adolescent Peer and Romantic Predictors of Youths’ Emotion Regulation in Early Adulthood Introduction David E. Szwedo David E. Szwedo,
Recruiting and Retaining “La Familia Latina” in Child and Adolescent Anxiety Treatment Research Armando A. Pina, Claudio D. Ortiz, and Wendy K. Silverman.
From Managing Emotions to Improving Relationships: Higher Quality Best Friendships Predicted from Earlier Emotion Regulation. Elenda T. Hessel, Megan M.
Project VIABLE - Direct Behavior Rating: Evaluating Behaviors with Positive and Negative Definitions Rose Jaffery 1, Albee T. Ongusco 3, Amy M. Briesch.
School-based Social Interactions of Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders Renee Hawkins, Ph.D., Laura Nabors Ph.D., Andrew Yockey, Stephanie Booker,
Discussion Results Introduction From Lateral to Leader: A Study of Preschoolers’ Relationships with Peers Erin Podgorski & Dr. Carin L. Neitzel, The University.
Can Pretty People Have Their Cake and Eat it Too? Positive and Negative Effects of Physical Attractiveness. Megan M. Schad, David E. Szwedo, Joanna M.
Problem-Solving Skills in Oppositional Youth: The Role of Parent – Child Synchrony ♦ Natalie E. Zazzara, Rachel L. Miller–Slough, M.S., Julie C. Dunsmore,
Longitudinal Effects of Mothers’ Encouragement and Discouragement of Positive Emotions on Children’s Task Behavior Deon Brown, Julie C. Dunsmore, Erika.
State University of New York at Geneseo
Mothers' Vocabulary and Autonomy-Granting Behaviors as Predictors of Gains in Children's Vocabulary Competence from Age 3 to Age 4 Sara L. Sohr-Preston.
Introduction Hypotheses Results Discussion Method
TREATMENT SENSITIVITY OF THE DYADIC PARENT-CHILD INTERACTION CODING SYSTEM-II Jenny Klein, B.S., Branlyn Werba, M.S., and Sheila Eyberg, Ph.D. University.
This research was supported by NIAAA K01AA
Introduction Results Hypotheses Discussion Method
Introduction Results Method Conclusions
Parents & Peers: Providing Teens with Different Tools to Develop Social Competence Meghan Costello1, Laura Sylke2, David Szwedo2, & Joseph Allen1 University.
Introduction Results Methods Conclusions
Birds of a Feather: Amplification Effects of Teen Best Friendships on Psychological Functioning Meghan Costello, Joseph P. Allen, & Rachel Narr University.
Introduction Results Conclusions Method
Introduction Results Conclusions Method
Introduction Results Hypotheses Discussion Method
The Importance of Positive Peer Relationships in Predicting Decreases in Adolescents’ Depressive Symptoms over Time Joanna M. Chango, Erin M. Miga, & Joseph.
Laura M. Sylke & David E. Szwedo James Madison University Introduction
Emily A. Davis & David E. Szwedo James Madison University Introduction
Lauren A. Barlotta & David E. Szwedo James Madison University
Prosocial Behaviors in Adolescence
Maddison Miles & David E. Szwedo James Madison University
General Social Competence (18)
Puberty Moderates Effects of the Parent-Child Relationship on
Presentation transcript:

Gender Differences in Buffering Stress Responses in Same-Sex Friend Dyads Sydney N. Pauling, Jenalee R. Doom, & Megan R. Gunnar Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota Introduction One important aspect of peer relationships involves the ability of peers to buffer an individual’s response to stress. Prior evidence in the field suggests that having a high quality friend present while recovering from a stress task is related to a faster return to baseline levels of the stress hormone cortisol in year olds (Calhoun et al., 2014). In pre-pubertal children, parents can lower levels of cortisol in response to stress (Hostinar et al., 2014). However, after puberty, parents are less potent stress buffers and youth must rely more on peer support (Hostinar et al., 2014). The purpose of this study was to investigate gender and age differences in friendship behaviors and to examine whether friendship behaviors predict the cortisol response to social stress. Specifically, do boys and girls differ in how they provide support to friends before a stressful task, and do these variations in support affect their friend’s physiological response to the task? Methods Participants 55 participants (33 female dyads) 29 participants 9-10 years of age (M = 10.0 yrs, SD =.44) 26 participants years of age (M = 15.8 yrs, SD =.46) Each participant brought a same-sex friend within 2 years of their age Procedure Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) consisting of public speaking task and math task in front of judges Peer assists participant in five minutes of speech preparation prior to TSST A coding scheme was then implemented to measure the various friendship behaviors that occurred during the speech preparation process Friendship Variables Inter-rater reliability was above α=.756 for all individual categories. Researchers coded for various friendship behaviors within the following categories: 1.Positive support a)Validation b)Sensitivity c)Time spent engaged d)Positive valence 2.Participant and peer anxiety (mean of these variables) 3.Humor/Distraction Factor (mean) a)Use of humor b)Distraction Cortisol Salivary cortisol, a stress hormone, was collected 7 times to measure each individual’s physiological response All sessions were conducted in the late afternoon due to diurnal rhythm of cortisol levels Medication and time since wake were used as co-variates Area under the curve (AUC) calculated to look at overall cortisol production throughout the session. Results Friendship Behaviors Age [t(50) = -.63, p =.53] and sex[t(50) = -.67, p =.51] did not predict peer/participant anxiety. There was a trend towards an age x sex interaction [t(50)= 1.95, p=.06]. In children, boys show less anxiety than girls (0.03). Sex [t(50) = -2.55, p =.01] predicted level of positive support during speech prep. Boys give less positive support. Age [t(50) = 1.79, p =.08] had a tendency to predict level of positive support. Adolescents tend to give more positive support. Age [t(50) = 2.37, p =.02] predicted how much humor/distraction was used, but sex [t(50) =.812, p =.421] did not. Adolescents use more humor/distraction than children. Cortisol (AUC) Medication and hours since wake did not predict cortisol in any of the analyses, p >.10. Sex did not predict cortisol in any of the analyses p>.10. Across analyses, the adolescents tended to produce more cortisol than children, p <.10. Greater participant and peer anxiety significantly predicted higher cortisol levels [t(49) = 2.06, p =.05]. Positive support was not related to cortisol [t(49) = -.09, p =.93]. Humor/distraction was not related to cortisol [t(49) = -1.4, p =.18]. Discussion This study suggests both age and sex differences in a variety of friendship behaviors: positive support, peer/participant anxiety, and humor/distraction. Statistically marginal effects indicate that larger n’s were needed in each age/sex group to adequately test our predictions. Peer support did not correlate with cortisol responses; although peer/participant anxiety did. Other measures of stress (e.g., HR) might have been more sensitive to peer support and should be examined.