Chapter 10: Perceiving Depth and Size

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Monocular and Binocular cues
Advertisements

Perception. Rules of Perceptual Organization Gestalt Psychologists The whole is more than the sum of its parts Closure Even if there are gaps in a picture.
Chapter 10: Perceiving Depth and Size
How Far Away Is It? Depth Perception
Perception Chapter 4.
Depth Cues Pictorial Depth Cues: aspects of 2D images that imply depth
Sources of Illusions Illusions occur because our perceptual system is structured to function as though our assumptions and hypotheses about the world around.
PERCEPTION Our ________________ of the stimuli coming in from the world around us.
PERCEPTION Chapter 4.5. Gestalt Principles  Gestalt principles are based on the idea that the whole is greater than the sum of the parts.  These principles.
Imaging Science FundamentalsChester F. Carlson Center for Imaging Science The Human Visual System Part 2: Perception.
Perception The process of organizing and interpreting information, enabling us to recognize meaningful objects and events.
Unit 4: Sensation & Perception
Extra Credit for Participating in Experiments Go to to sign upwww.tatalab.ca We are recruiting for a variety of experiments including basic.
Perception.
PSYC 1000 Lecture 21. Selective Attention: Stroop.
Thinking About Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behavior Charles T. Blair-Broeker Randal M. Ernst.
Chapter 8: Vision in three dimensions Basic issue: How do we construct a three-dimension visual experience from two- dimensional visual input? Important.
Chapter 6 Opener. Figure 6.1 The Euclidean geometry of the three-dimensional world turns into something quite different on the curved, two-dimensional.
2002/02/12PSYC202, Term 2, Copyright Jason Harrison Recovery of World Structure: Art and Image.
3/23/2005 © Dr. Zachary Wartell 1 Depth and Size Perception.
Imaging Science FundamentalsChester F. Carlson Center for Imaging Science Binocular Vision and The Perception of Depth.
DEPTH AND SIZE PERCEPTION Problems for Perceiving Depth and Size Oculomotor Cues Monocular Cues Binocular Disparity Size Constancy.
Sensation and Perception - depth.ppt © 2001 Dr. Laura Snodgrass, Ph.D. Depth Perception Four theoretical approaches –Cue theory unconscious calculation.
Monocular vs. Binocular View Monocular view: one eye only! Many optical instruments are designed from one eye view. Binocular view: two eyes with each.
Reading Gregory 24 th Pinker 26 th. Seeing Depth What’s the big problem with seeing depth ?
Careers for Psychology and Neuroscience Majors Oct. 19th5-7pm in SU 300 Ballroom B.
Thinking About Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behavior Charles T. Blair-Broeker Randal M. Ernst.
DEPTH PRINCIPLES The ability to accurately estimate the distance of objects and therefore perceive the world in three dimensions.
Biopsychological Domain
By Andrea Rees. Gestalt Principles 1) Closure 2) Proximity 3) Similarity 4) Figure VISUAL PERCEPTION PRINCIPLES OVERVIEW Depth Principles Binocular 1)
Chapter 6: Sensation and Perception 1. Some Definitions: Sensation - process used by sense receptors to receive and store information from environment.
BY JESSIE PARKER VISUAL PERCEPTION PRINCIPLES. VISUAL PERCEPTION Visual perception is the ability to interpret the surrounding environment by processing.
Perception The process of organizing and interpreting information, enabling us to recognize meaningful objects and events.
VIEWING THE WORLD IN COLOR. COLOR A psychological interpretation Based on wavelength, amplitude, and purity Humans can discriminate among c. 10 million.
1 Perception, Illusion and VR HNRS 299, Spring 2008 Lecture 8 Seeing Depth.
Chapter 6 Section 2: Vision. What we See Stimulus is light –Visible light comes from sun, stars, light bulbs, & is reflected off objects –Travels in the.
Vision Part 2 Theories on processing colors. Objectives: The Student Will Compare and contrast color theories (VENN) Explain the Gestalt Theory List your.
Physiological Depth Cues – Convergence. Physiological Depth Cues – Convergence – small angle of convergence = far away – large angle of convergence =
Perception Chapter 5.
Perception. The process of organizing and interpreting sensory information.
Chapter 10: Perceiving Depth and Size. Overview of Questions How can we see far into the distance based on the flat image of the retina? Why do we see.
Perception The process of organizing and interpreting information, enabling us to recognize meaningful objects and events.
Vision: Distance & Size Perception. Useful terms: ● Egocentric distance: distance from you to an object. ● Relative distance: distance between two objects.
Warm Up #8 How many pegs do you see?
Depth Perception and Perceptional Illusions. Depth Perception The use of visual cues to perceive the distance or three-dimensional characteristics of.
Perception 1. Inattentional Blindness Challenge: Count the number of passes the white shirts pass! VideoVideo (2mins) Video Type of selective attention.
Visual Perception Principles Visual perception principles are ‘rules’ that we apply to visual information to assist our organisation and interpretation.
Perception The process of organizing and interpreting information, enabling us to recognize meaningful objects and events.
Thinking About Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behavior 2e Charles T. Blair-Broeker Randal M. Ernst.
Depth Perception Kimberley A. Clow
How Far Away Is It? Depth Perception
Sensation and Perception
Perception: Uses top-down processing The process of organizing and interpreting sensory information Module 10.
Perception: Uses top-down processing The process of organizing and interpreting sensory information Module 10.
Perception and VR MONT 104S, Fall 2008 Lecture 8 Seeing Depth
Perception. The process of organizing, and interpreting sensory information enabling us to recognize meaningful objects and events Example:
Visual Perception. What is Visual Perception? Visual perception are rules we apply to visual information to assist our organisation and interpretation.
VISUAL PERCEPTION PRINCIPLES By Mikayla. VISUAL PERCEPTION PRINCIPLES  Gestalt principles 1.Closure 2.Proximity 3.Similarity 4.Figure-ground  Depth.
Perception of Depth. Cues to depth: 1. Oculomotor 2. Monocular 3. Binocular.
Perception and memory 1- Perception 2- Memory. What is perception? A process by which the brain analyses and makes sense out of incoming sensory information.
Depth Perception Allows you to see the world in three dimensions (3D) Allows you to see the world in three dimensions (3D)
Thinking About Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behavior
Visual Perception Principles
How Far Away Is It? Depth Perception
Perception We have previously examined the sensory processes by which stimuli are encoded. Now we will examine the ultimate purpose of sensory information.
How Far Away Is It? Depth Perception
Perceptual Organization
Module 19 – Visual Organization and Interpretation
Introduction to Visual Perception
Gestalt The “whole,” or the organizational patterns that we tend to perceive Gestalt psychologists stressed that the whole is greater than the sum of its.
Presentation transcript:

Chapter 10: Perceiving Depth and Size

Figure 10.1 (a) In the scene, the house is farther away than the tree, but images of points H on the house and T on the tree both fall on the two-dimensional surface of the retina on the back of the eye. (b) These two points on the retinal image, considered by themselves, do not tell us the distances of the house and the tree. Figure 10-1 p228

Cue Approach to Depth Perception This approach focuses on information in the retinal image that is correlated with depth in the scene. Occlusion We learn the connection between the cue and depth. The association becomes automatic through repeat exposure. 3

Oculomotor Cues Oculomotor cues are based on sensing the position of the eyes and muscle tension Convergence - inward movement of the eyes when we focus on nearby objects Accommodation - change in the shape of the lens when we focus on objects at different distances 4

Figure 10.2 (a) Convergence of the eyes occurs when a person looks at something that is very close. (b) The eyes look straight ahead when the person observes something that is far away. Figure 10-2 p229

Monocular Cues Monocular cues come from one eye Pictorial cues - sources of depth information that come from 2-D images, such as pictures Occlusion - when one object partially covers another Relative height - objects below the horizon that are higher in the field of vision are more distant Objects above the horizon lower in the visual field are more distant 6

Monocular Cues - continued Relative size - when objects are equal size, the closer one will take up more of your visual field Perspective convergence - parallel lines appear to come together in the distance Familiar size - distance information based on our knowledge of object size 7

Figure 10.3 (a) A scene in Tucson, Arizona, containing a number of depth cues: occlusion (the cactus on the right occludes the hill, which occludes the mountain); relative height (the far motorcycle is higher in the field of view than the closer motorcycle); relative size (the far motorcycle and telephone pole are smaller than the near ones); and perspective convergence (the sides of the road converge in the distance). (b) 1, 2, and 3 indicate the increasing height in the field of view of the bases of the motorcycles and the far telephone pole, which reveals that being higher in the field of view causes objects on the ground to appear farther away; 4 and 5 reveal that being lower in the field of view causes objects in the sky to appear farther away. Figure 10-3 p229

Figure 10. 4 Pietro Perugino. Christ Handing the Keys to St Figure 10.4 Pietro Perugino. Christ Handing the Keys to St. Peter (Sistine Chapel). The convergence of lines on the plaza illustrates perspective convergence. The sizes of the people in the foreground and middle ground illustrate relative size. Figure 10-4 p230

Figure 10.5 Drawings of the stimuli used in Epstein’s (1965) familiar-size experiment. The actual stimuli were photographs that were all the same size as a real quarter. Figure 10-5 p230

Monocular Cues - continued Atmospheric perspective - distance objects are fuzzy and have a blue tint Texture gradient - equally spaced elements are more closely packed as distance increases Shadows - indicate where objects are located Enhance 3-D of objects 11

Figure 10.6 A scene on the coast of Maine showing the effect of atmospheric perspective. Figure 10-6 p231

Figure 10. 7 Texture gradients created by marathon runners and flowers Figure 10.7 Texture gradients created by marathon runners and flowers. The increasing fineness of texture as distance increases enhances the perception of depth. Figure 10-7 p231

Figure 10.8 (a) Where are the spheres located in relation to the checkerboard? (b) Adding shadows makes their location clearer. Figure 10-8 p231

Figure 10.9 (a) Early morning shadows emphasize the mountain’s contours. (b) When the sun is overhead, the shadows vanish, and it becomes more difficult to see the mountain’s contours. Figure 10-9 p232

Covering an object is deletion Uncovering an object is accretion Motion-Produced Cues Motion parallax - close objects in direction of movement glide rapidly past but objects in the distance appear to move slowly Deletion and accretion - objects are covered or uncovered as we move relative to them Covering an object is deletion Uncovering an object is accretion 16

Figure 10.10 One eye moving past (a) a nearby tree; (b) a far-away house. Because the tree is closer, its image moves farther across the retina than the image of the house. Figure 10-10 p233

Table 10-1 p233

Binocular Depth Information Stereoscopic depth perception Differences between 2D and 3D movies “Stereo Sue” Strabismus 19

Seeing Depth With Two Eyes Differences between 2D and 3D movies “Stereo Sue” Strabismus 20

Figure 10.12 Location of images on the retina for the “Two Eyes: Two Viewpoints” demonstration. See text for explanation. Figure 10-12 p234

Figure 10.13 (a) When we view a two-dimensional movie, the left and right eyes receive essentially the same images, so depth is indicated only by monocular pictorial depth cues. (b) When viewing a 3-D movie, the left and right eyes receive different images, so stereoscopic depth perception occurs. Figure 10-13 p235

Binocular disparity - difference in images from two eyes Difference can be described by examining corresponding points on the two retinas The horopter - imaginary sphere that passes through the point of focus Objects on the horopter fall on corresponding points on the two retinas 23

Binocular Disparity - continued Objects that do not fall on the horopter fall on noncorresponding points These points make disparate images. The angle between these points is the absolute disparity. The amount of disparity indicates how far an object is from the horopter. Relative disparity is the difference between the absolute disparity of two objects. 24

Figure 10. 14 Corresponding points on the two retinas Figure 10.14 Corresponding points on the two retinas. To determine corresponding points, imagine that the left eye is slid on top of the right eye. F indicates the fovea, where the image of an object occurs when an observer looks directly at the object, and A is a point in the peripheral retina. Images on the fovea always fall on corresponding points. Notice that the A’s, which also fall on corresponding points, are the same distance from the fovea in the left and right eyes. Figure 10-14 p236

Figure 10.15 (a) An observer looking at Julie’s face, with a tree off to the side. (b) The observer’s eyes, showing where the images of Julie and the tree fall on each eye. Julie’s images fall on the fovea, so they are on corresponding points. The arrows indicate that the tree’s images are located the same distances from the fovea in the two eyes, so they are also on corresponding points. The dashed blue line is the horopter. The images of objects that are on the horopter fall on corresponding points. Figure 10-15 p236

Figure 10.16 (a) When the observer looks at Julie, Julie’s images fall on corresponding points. Bill’s images fall on noncorresponding points. The angle of disparity, indicated by the red arrow, is determined by measuring the angle between where the corresponding point for Bill’s image would be located (black dot) and where Bill’s image is actually located (red dot). (b) Dave has been added to Figure 10.16a. Dave’s angle of disparity (blue arrow) is greater than Bill’s, because Dave is located farther from the horopter. Figure 10-16 p237

Figure 10.17 Absolute disparities change when an observer’s gaze shifts from one place to another. (a) When the observer looks at Julie, the disparity of her images is zero. Bill’s angle of disparity is indicated by the arrow. (b) When the observer looks at Bill, the disparity of Bill’s images becomes zero. Julie’s angle of disparity is indicated by the arrow. Because one of the disparities in each pair is zero, the arrows indicate the difference in disparity between Julie’s and Bill’s images. Note that the difference in disparity is the same in (a) and (b). This means that the relative disparity of Julie and Bill remains the same as the observer looks at different places. Figure 10-17 p238

Disparity (Geometrical) Created Stereopsis (Perceptual) Stereopsis - depth information provided by binocular disparity Stereoscope uses two pictures from slightly different viewpoints. Random-dot stereogram has two identical patterns with one shifted in position. 29

Figure 10.18 Disparity is related to geometry—the locations of images on the retina. Stereopsis is related to perception—the experience of depth created by disparity. Figure 10-18 p238

Figure 10. 19 The two images of a stereoscopic photograph Figure 10.19 The two images of a stereoscopic photograph. The difference between the two images, such as the distances between the front cactus and the window in the two views, creates retinal disparity. This creates a perception of depth when the left image is viewed by the left eye and the right image is viewed by the right eye. Figure 10-19 p239

Disparity (Geometrical) Created Stereopsis (Perceptual) - continued Three types of 3D TV Passive – polarized glasses Active – electronic shutter glasses Lenticular – mini lenses on screen, no glasses needed 32

Figure 10.20 Three types of 3-D TV. See text for details. Figure 10-20 p239

Figure 10. 21 (a) A random-dot stereogram Figure 10.21 (a) A random-dot stereogram. (b) The principle for constructing the stereogram. See text for an explanation. Figure 10-21 p240

The Correspondence Problem How does the visual system match images from the two eyes? Matches may be made by specific features of objects. This may not work for objects like random-dot stereograms. A satisfactory answer has not yet been proposed. 35

The Physiology of Binocular Depth Perception Neurons have been found that respond best to binocular disparity. These are called binocular depth cells or disparity selective cells. These cells respond best to a specific degree of absolute disparity between images on the right and left retinas. Disparity tuning curve 36

Figure 10.22 Disparity tuning curve for a neuron sensitive to absolute disparity. This curve indicates the neural response that occurs when stimuli presented to the left and right eyes create different amounts of disparity. Figure 10-22 p241

Figure 10.23 The three relationships in the perceptual process, as applied to binocular disparity. We have described experiments relating disparity to perception (green arrow) and relating disparity to physiological responding (orange arrow). The final step is to determine the relationship between physiological responses to disparity and perception (red arrow). This has been studied by selective rearing, which eliminates disparity-selective neurons, as well as by other methods described in the text. Figure 10-23 p242

The Physiology of Binocular Depth Perception - continued Experiment by Blake and Hirsch Cats were reared by alternating vision between two eyes. Results showed that they: had few binocular neurons. were unable to use binocular disparity to perceive depth. 39

The Physiology of Binocular Depth Perception - continued Experiment by DeAngelis et al. Monkey trained to indicate depth from disparate images. Disparity-selective neurons were activated by this process. Experimenter used microstimulation to activate different disparity-selective neurons. Monkey shifted judgment to the artificially stimulated disparity. 40

Figure 10.24 While the monkey was observing a random-dot stereogram, DeAngelis and coworkers (1998) stimulated neurons in the monkey’s cortex that were sensitive to a particular amount of disparity. This stimulation shifted the monkey’s perception of the depth of the field of dots from position 1 to position 2. Figure 10-24 p242

Distance and size perception are interrelated Perceiving Size Distance and size perception are interrelated Experiment by Holway and Boring Observer was at the intersection of two hallways. A luminous test circle was in the right hallway placed from 10 to 120 feet away. A luminous comparison circle was in the left hallway at 10 feet away. 42

Figure 10.25 When a helicopter pilot loses the ability to perceive distance in a “whiteout,” a small box that is close can be mistaken for a truck that is far away. Figure 10-25 p243

Perceiving Size - continued Experiment by Holway and Boring On each trial the observer was to adjust the diameter of the test circle to match the comparison. Test stimuli all had same visual angle (angle of object relative to the observer’s eye). Visual angle depends on both the size of the object and the distance from the observer. 44

Figure 10. 26 Setup of Holway and Boring’s (1941) experiment Figure 10.26 Setup of Holway and Boring’s (1941) experiment. The observer changes the diameter of the comparison circle in the left corridor to match his or her perception of the size of test circles presented in the right corridor. Each test circle has a visual angle of 1 degree and is presented separately. This diagram is not drawn to scale. The actual distance of the far test circle was 100 feet. Figure 10-26 p243

Figure 10.27 (a) The visual angle depends on the size of the stimulus (the woman in this example) and its distance from the observer. (b) When the woman moves closer to the observer, the visual angle and the size of the image on the retina increase. This example shows that halving the distance between the stimulus and observer doubles the size of the image on the retina. Figure 10-27 p244

Figure 10.28 The “thumb” method of determining the visual angle of an object. When the thumb is at arm’s length, it has a visual angle of about 2 degrees. The woman’s thumb covers the width of her phone, so the visual angle of the phone, from the woman’s point of view, is 2 degrees. Note that the visual angle will change if the distance between the woman and the phone changes. Figure 10-28 p244

Figure 10.29 The visual angle between the two fingers is the same as the visual angle of the Eiffel tower. Figure 10-29 p245

Perceiving Size - continued Part 1 of the experiment provided observers with depth cues. Judgments of size were based on physical size. Part 2 of the experiment provided no depth information. Judgments of size were based on size of the retinal images. 49

Figure 10. 30 Results of Holway and Boring’s experiment Figure 10.30 Results of Holway and Boring’s experiment. The dashed line labeled “physical size” is the result that would be expected if the observers adjusted the diameter of the comparison circle to match the actual diameter of each test circle. The line labeled “visual angle” is the result that would be expected if the observers adjusted the diameter of the comparison circle to match the visual angle of each test circle. Figure 10-30 p245

Figure 10.31 The moon’s disk almost exactly covers the sun during an eclipse because the sun and the moon have the same visual angles. Figure 10-31 p246

Perception of an object’s size remains relatively constant. Size Constancy Perception of an object’s size remains relatively constant. This effect remains even if the size of the retinal image changes. Size-distance scaling equation S = K (R X D) The changes in distance and retinal size balance each other 52

Size Constancy- continued Emmert’s law: Retinal size of an afterimage remains constant. Perceived size will change depending on distance of projection. This follows the size-distance scaling equation. 53

Figure 10.32 All of the palm trees appear to be the same size when viewed in the environment, even though the farther ones have a smaller visual angle. Figure 10-32 p246

Figure 10.33 The principle behind the observation that the size of an afterimage increases as the afterimage is viewed against more distant surfaces. Figure 10-33 p247

Figure 10.34 The size of this wheel becomes apparent when it is compared to the person. If the wheel were seen in isolation, it would be difficult to know that it is so large. Figure 10-34 p248

Figure 10. 35 Two cylinders resting on a texture gradient Figure 10.35 Two cylinders resting on a texture gradient. The fact that the bases of both cylinders cover the same portion of a paving stone indicates that the two cylinders are the same size. Figure 10-35 p248

Nonveridical perception occurs during visual illusions. Müller-Lyer illusion: Straight lines with inward fins appear shorter than straight lines with outward fins. Lines are actually the same length. 58

Why does this illusion occur? Misapplied size-constancy scaling: Müller-Lyer Illusion Why does this illusion occur? Misapplied size-constancy scaling: Size constancy scaling that works in 3-D is misapplied for 2-D objects. Observers unconsciously perceive the fins as belonging to outside and inside corners. Outside corners would be closer and inside corners would be further away. 59

Figure 10. 36 The Müller-Lyer illusion Figure 10.36 The Müller-Lyer illusion. Both lines are actually the same length. Figure 10-36 p249

Figure 10.37 According to Gregory (1966), the Müller-Lyer line on the left corresponds to an outside corner, and the line on the right corresponds to an inside corner. Note that the two vertical lines are the same length (measure them!). Figure 10-37 p249

Müller-Lyer Illusion - continued Since the retinal images are the same, the lines must be different sizes. Problems with this explanation: The “dumbbell” version shows the same perception even though there are no “corners.” The illusion also occurs for some 3-D displays. 62

Figure 10. 38 The “dumbbell” version of the Müller-Lyer illusion Figure 10.38 The “dumbbell” version of the Müller-Lyer illusion. As in the original Müller-Lyer illusion, the two straight lines are actually the same length. Figure 10-38 p250

Figure 10. 39 A three-dimensional Müller-Lyer illusion Figure 10.39 A three-dimensional Müller-Lyer illusion. The 2-foot- high wooden “fins” stand on the floor. Although the distances x and y are the same, distance y appears larger, just as in the two-dimensional Müller-Lyer illusion. Figure 10-39 p250

Müller-Lyer Illusion - continued Another possible explanation: Conflicting cues theory - our perception of line length depends on: The actual length of the vertical lines The overall length of the figure The conflicting cues are integrated into a compromise perception of the length. 65

Figure 10. 40 An alternate version of the Müller-Lyer illusion Figure 10.40 An alternate version of the Müller-Lyer illusion. We perceive that the distance between the dots in (a) is less than the distance in (b), even though the distances are the same. (From Day, 1989.) Figure 10-40 p250

One possible explanation is misapplied size-constancy scaling. Ponzo Illusion Horizontal rectangular objects are placed over railroad tracks in a picture. The far rectangle appears larger than the closer rectangle but both are the same size. One possible explanation is misapplied size-constancy scaling. 67

Figure 10. 41 The Ponzo (or railroad track) illusion Figure 10.41 The Ponzo (or railroad track) illusion. The two animals are the same length on the page (measure them), but the upper one appears larger. Figure 10-41 p251

Two people of equal size appear very different in size in this room. The Ames Room Two people of equal size appear very different in size in this room. The room is constructed so that: The shape looks like a normal room when viewed with one eye. The actual shape has the left corner twice as far away as the right corner. 69

Figure 10.42 The Ames room. Both women are actually the same height, but the woman on the right appears taller because of the distorted shape of the room. (The Exploratorium/S. Schwartzenberg.) Figure 10-42 p251

Figure 10. 43 The Ames room, showing its true shape Figure 10.43 The Ames room, showing its true shape. The person on the left is actually almost twice as far away from the observer as the person on the right; however, when the room is viewed through the peephole, this difference in distance is not seen. In order for the room to look normal when viewed through the peephole, it is necessary to enlarge the left side of the room. Figure 10-43 p251

Video: The Ames Room 72

The Ames Room - continued One possible explanation - size-distance scaling Observer thinks the room is normal. Women would be at same distance. Woman on the left has smaller visual angle (R). Due to the perceived distance (D) being the same her perceived size (S) is smaller 73

The Ames Room - continued Another possible explanation - relative size Perception of size depends on size relative to other objects. One woman fills the distance between the top and bottom of the room. The other woman only fills part of the distance Thus, the woman on the right appears taller 74

One possible explanation: Moon Illusion The moon appears larger on the horizon than when it is higher in the sky. One possible explanation: Apparent-distance theory - horizon moon is surrounded by depth cues while moon higher in the sky has none. Horizon is perceived as further away than the sky - called “flattened heavens”. 75

Moon Illusion - continued Since the moon in both cases has the same visual angle, it must appear larger at the horizon. Another possible explanation: Angular size-contrast theory - the moon appears smaller when surrounded by larger objects Thus, the large expanse of the sky makes it appear smaller Actual explanation may be a combination of a number of cues. Other factors are: atmospheric perspective, color, and oculomotor cues. 76

Figure 10.44 An artist’s conception of how the moon is perceived when it is on the horizon and when it is high in the sky. Note that the visual angle of the horizon moon is depicted as larger than the visual angle of the moon high in the sky. This is because the picture is simulating the illusion. In the environment, the visual angles of the two moons are the same. Figure 10-44 p252

Figure 10.45 When observers are asked to consider the sky as a surface and to compare the distance to the horizon (H) and the distance to the top of the sky on a clear moonless night, they usually say that the horizon appears farther away. This results in the “flattened heavens” shown here. Figure 10-45 p252

Depth Perception Across Species Animals use the range of cues that humans use. Frontal eyes, which result in overlapping fields of view, are necessary for binocular disparity. Lateral eyes, which do not result in overlapping fields of view, provide a wider view. This is important for watching for predators. 79

Figure 10.46 (a) Frontal eyes, such as those of the cat, have overlapping fields of view that provide good depth perception. (b) Lateral eyes, such as those of the rabbit, provide a panoramic view but poorer depth perception. Figure 10-46 p253

Depth Perception in Other Species - continued Locusts use motion parallax to judge distance. Bats use echolocation to judge the distance of objects in the dark. They emit sounds and note the interval between when they send them and when they receive the echo. 81

Figure 10.47 When a bat sends out its pulses, it receives echoes from a number of objects in the environment. This figure shows the echoes received by the bat from (a) a nearby moth; (b) a tree located about 2 meters away; and (c) a house located about 4 meters away. The echoes from more distant objects take longer to return. The bat locates the positions of objects in the environment by sensing how long it takes the echoes to return. Figure 10-47 p254

Infant Depth Perception Binocular disparity becomes function early Binocular disparity Binocularly fixate Pictorial depth cues become function later Depth from familiar size Depth from cast shadows 83

Figure 10. 48 The setup used by Fox et al Figure 10.48 The setup used by Fox et al. (1980) to test infants’ ability to use binocular disparity information. If the infant can use disparity information to see depth, he or she sees a rectangle moving back and forth in front of the screen. Figure 10-48 p255

Figure 10.49 Stimuli for Granrud, Haake, and Yonas’s (1985) familiar size experiment. See text for details. Figure 10-49 p255

Figure 10.50 Stimuli presented to 5- and 7-month-old children in Yonas and Granrud’s (2006) cast shadow experiment Figure 10-50 p256

Video: Size Constancy & Infants 87