Randomized Single-Case Intervention Designs Joel R

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Overview of Withdrawal Designs
Advertisements

Chapter 9 Overview of Alternating Treatments Designs.
Experimental Research Designs
Experimental Design: Single-Participant Designs/ The Operant Approach.
PTP 560 Research Methods Week 4 Thomas Ruediger, PT.
Single -Subject Designs - Ch 5 “Data collection allows teachers to make statements about the direction and magnitude of behavioral changes” (p. 116). In.
Quasi-Experimental Designs
Questions  In a correlation research paper do the authors note the predictive variable and the criterion variable?  Is it common to combine different.
Chapter 12 Single-Case Research Designs ♣ ♣ Introduction   Single-Case Designs   Methodological Considerations in Using Single- Case Designs   Criteria.
Introduction to Research
Statement of the Problem Goal Establishes Setting of the Problem hypothesis Additional information to comprehend fully the meaning of the problem scopedefinitionsassumptions.
Educational Research by John W. Creswell. Copyright © 2002 by Pearson Education. All rights reserved. Slide 1 Chapter 11 Experimental and Quasi-experimental.
Single-Subject Research
L1 Chapter 11 Experimental and Quasi- experimental Designs Dr. Bill Bauer.
Experimental Research
Single-Subject Designs
Experiments and Observational Studies.  A study at a high school in California compared academic performance of music students with that of non-music.
Copyright © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved. Doing Research in Behavior Modification Chapter 22.
Doing Research in Behavior Modification
بسم الله الرحمن الرحيم * this presentation about :- “experimental design “ * Induced to :- Dr Aidah Abu Elsoud Alkaissi * Prepared by :- 1)-Hamsa karof.
Chapter 11 Research Methods in Behavior Modification.
Single-Case Research: Standards for Design and Analysis Thomas R. Kratochwill University of Wisconsin-Madison.
Copyright © 2007 Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Pearson Addison-Wesley Chapter 13 Experiments and Observational Studies.
Statistical Randomization Techniques for Single-Case Intervention Data Statistical Randomization Techniques for Single-Case Intervention Data Joel R. Levin.
Single- Subject Research Designs
Moving from Single-Case to Conventional Group Intervention Designs, and Vice Versa Joel R. Levin University of Arizona.
INTERNATIONAL SOCIETY FOR TECHNOLOGY IN EDUCATION working together to improve education with technology Using Evidence for Educational Technology Success.
Randomization: A Missing Component of the Single-Case Research Methodological Standards Adapted from Kratochwill, T. R., & Levin, J. R. (2010). Enhancing.
Classroom-Based Applications of Single-Case Designs: Methodological and Statistical Issues Joel R. Levin University of Arizona.
Current Methodological Issues in Single Case Research David Rindskopf, City University of New York Rob Horner, University of Oregon.
Quantitative and Qualitative Approaches
Educational Research: Competencies for Analysis and Application, 9 th edition. Gay, Mills, & Airasian © 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved.
Single-Subject Experimental Research
Chapter 11 Overview of Changing Criterion Design.
@ 2012 Wadsworth, Cengage Learning Chapter 10 Extending the Logic of Experimentation: Within-Subjects and Matched-Subjects 2012 Wadsworth,
Experimental Design ã Dependent variable (DV): Variable observed to determine the effects of an experimental manipulation (behavior) ã Independent variable.
McGraw-Hill/Irwin Copyright © 2011 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. All rights reserved. Using Single-Subject Designs.
Statistical Models for the Analysis of Single-Case Intervention Data Introduction to:  Regression Models  Multilevel Models.
Random Thoughts On Enhancing the Scientific Credibility of Single-Case Intervention Research: Randomization to the Rescue Thomas R. Kratochwill and Joel.
Educational Research: Competencies for Analysis and Application, 9 th edition. Gay, Mills, & Airasian © 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. All rights reserved.
SOCW 671 # 8 Single Subject/System Designs Intro to Sampling.
Experimental Research Methods in Language Learning Chapter 4 Experimental Research Designs.
Introduction to Research. Purpose of Research Evidence-based practice Validate clinical practice through scientific inquiry Scientific rational must exist.
Statistical Randomization Techniques for Single-Case Intervention Data Statistical Randomization Techniques for Single-Case Intervention Data Joel R. Levin.
Single-Subject and Correlational Research Bring Schraw et al.
Brief Comments on Single-Case Effect Sizes and Meta-Analyses* Joel R. Levin University of Arizona *Based, in part, on Kratochwill, T. R., & Levin, J. R.
Single- Subject Research Designs
Overview of the Week’s Small-Group Activity Schedule.
IES Project Director’s Meeting June 2010 Rob Horner University of Oregon.
Unit 8.  Program improvement or appraisal  Assessing the value of a program  Measuring the efficacy of particular components of a program  Meeting.
IES Single-Case Research Institute: Training Visual Analysis Rob Horner University of Oregon
Masked Visual Analysis (MVA) A method to ensure control of the Type I error rate when visually analyzing single-case studies.
Project VIABLE - Direct Behavior Rating: Evaluating Behaviors with Positive and Negative Definitions Rose Jaffery 1, Albee T. Ongusco 3, Amy M. Briesch.
Reading is a fundamental skill for success in school. Thus, providing effective intervention to children underperforming in a standard classroom is critical.
1 Negative Results and Publication Bias in Single- Case Research Applications of the WWC Standards in Literature Reviews.
DAY 2 Visual Analysis of Single-Case Intervention Data Tom Kratochwill
Chapter 11: Quasi-Experimental and Single Case Experimental Designs
Individual Consultation
Joel R. Levin University of Arizona
Statistical Randomization Techniques for Single-Case Intervention Data and Introduction to ExPRT Statistical Software Joel R. Levin University of.
11 Single-Case Research Designs.
Research Methods in Behavior Change Programs
Masked Visual Analysis (MVA)
Research Methods: Concepts and Connections First Edition
DAY 2 Single-Case Design Drug Intervention Research Tom Kratochwill
Randomization: A Missing Component of the Single-Case Research Methodological Standards Joel R. Levin University of Arizona Adapted from Kratochwill, T.
Correlated-Groups and Single-Subject Designs
Small-n Designs.
DAY 2 Single-Case Design Drug Intervention Research Tom Kratochwill
Masked Visual Analysis (MVA)
Presentation transcript:

Randomized Single-Case Intervention Designs Joel R Randomized Single-Case Intervention Designs Joel R. Levin University of Arizona Adapted from Kratochwill, T. R., & Levin, J. R. (2010). Enhancing the scientific credibility of single-case intervention research: Randomization to the rescue. Psychological Methods, 15, 122-144.

Major Purpose of this Presentation To broaden your minds by introducing you to new and exciting, scientifically credible single-case intervention design possibilities. To let you know that these procedures are becoming increasingly acceptable to single-case intervention researchers and are beginning to appear in the SCD research literature. Whether YOU ever choose to adopt them in your own SCD research (after this week!) is entirely up to you.

Why Random? Internal Validity Elevates the status of single-case research by increasing the scientific credibility of its methodology Statistical-Conclusion Validity Legitimizes the conduct of various statistical tests and one’s interpretation of the results

Traditional Single-Case Designs Basic design (AB) Reversal (or withdrawal or “operant”) design (ABAB) Alternating and simultaneous treatment designs Multiple-probe design Changing criterion design Multiple-baseline design

Four Single-Case Design-and-Analysis Randomization Variations Phase/Intervention Randomization (within cases) 2. Intervention Randomization (between cases) 3. Start-Point Randomization 4. Case Randomization

Days/Weeks/Months/Sessions Traditional Basic Design (AB)

1. Phase/Intervention Randomization (Within Cases) With phase randomization, the order in which the A and B phases are administered is randomly determined for each case (e.g., participant, pair, small group, classroom).

AB Design “[I]nstead of automatically administering the two phases in an AB order, one could randomly determine which phase should come first.” (Levin, Marascuilo, & Hubert, 1978) Problems with randomizing phases? If A is a true baseline condition, logically it should be administered first. Problems with not randomizing phases? If A and B are two alternative intervention conditions, potential problems include both time-related effects and carryover effects. Levin, J. R., Marascuilo, L. A., & Hubert, L. J. (1978). N = nonparametric randomization tests. In T. R. Kratochwill (Ed.), Single subject research: Strategies for evaluating change (pp. 167-196). New York: Academic Press.

Traditional (“Systematic”) ABAB Reversal Design With One Experimental Case

ABAB Design Addresses time and carryover effects to some extent. “[H]owever, systematic assignment (A preceding B in each pair) is not the same as random assignment (either within each pair or within the entire experiment).” (Levin et al., 1978) Other problems include Hawthorne/novelty and expectancy effects. Edgington, E. S. (1992). Nonparametric tests for single-case experiments. In T. R. Kratochwill & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Single-case research design and analysis (pp.133-157). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Onghena, P. (1992). Randomization tests for extensions and variation of ABAB single-case experimental designs: A rejoinder. Behavioral Assessment, 14, 153-171.

Randomized Phase Designs For ABAB…AB and alternating-treatment designs, there are two basic types of phase randomization: simple and blocked. With simple randomization, the only constraint is that there be equal numbers of A and B phases in the design. With blocked randomization, additional constraints are imposed to control for order effects.

One Potentially Palatable Solution? Prior to initiating the formal AB intervention study, include one or more mandatory baseline (adaptation, warmup) observations (A'). The administration order of the subsequent A and B phases is then randomly determined. The A' phase is not considered to be part of the actual study’s design or analysis.

Randomized Alternating Treatment Design (ATD) With One Case, Two Within-Case Conditions, and 13 Time Periods (Seven Mornings and Six Afternoons)

Randomized ABAB . . . AB Design With One Case, Two Within-Case Conditions, and 10 Time Periods 5 A and 5 B)

Replicated Randomized ABAB Replicated Randomized ABAB . . . AB Design With Four Cases, Two Within-Case Conditions, and 10 Time Periods (5 A and 5 B)

2. Intervention Randomization (Between Cases) In some “between-case” multiple-intervention single-case designs, Treatment X (a control or intervention condition) is administered to one or more cases and Treatment Y (an alternative intervention condition) is administered to other cases. With intervention randomization, which cases receive Treatment X and which receive Treatment Y is randomly determined. [Examples of intervention randomization are presented in later slides.]

A Two-Intervention (Between Cases) Example

Time Out for an Introduction to “Intervention Start Points” (To Be Returned to Throughout the Institute) The intervention start point [and subsequent transition points] is [are] “response guided” – preferred by many traditional SCD researchers later discussion of adopting this approach with the assistance of a “masked visual analyst (MVA)” The intervention start point [and subsequent transition points] is/are designated on an a priori basis by the researcher – preferred by traditional methodologists The intervention start point [and subsequent transition points] is [are] randomly selected from a set of potential points that is designated as “acceptable” by the researcher – preferred by “new-age” SCD methodologists

3. Start-Point Randomization With start-point randomization, the actual A-to-B transition (“intervention start point”) is randomly selected from a set of researcher-designated “acceptable” (or “potential”) start points. This type of randomization can be implemented in single-case designs where A and B are either baseline and intervention conditions or two different intervention conditions.

AB Design With One Case (“Unit”), Two Within-Series Intervention Conditions, 20 Time Periods, and 13 Potential Intervention Start Points

Replicated AB Design With Three Cases (“Units”), Two Within-Series Intervention Conditions, 20 Time Periods, and 13 Potential Intervention Points for Each Case Marascuilo, L. A., & Busk, P. L. (1988). Combining statistics for multiple-baseline AB and replicated ABAB designs across subjects. Behavioral Assessment, 10, 1-28.

4. Case Randomization With case randomization, cases are randomly assigned to the different replication positions within the design. Multiple-baseline designs, with their systematically staggered intervention start points, are uniquely suited to this type of randomization.

Traditional Multiple-Baseline Design Across Participants

Randomized Multiple-Baseline Design Across Cases (“Units”) With 5 Randomized Cases, Two Within-Series Intervention Conditions (A and B), 10 Time Periods, and a Staggered Intervention Introduction of One Time Period Kratochwill, T. R., & Levin, J. R. (1978). What time-series designs may have to offer educational researchers. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 3, 273-329. Wampold, B. E., & Worsham, N. L. (1986). Randomization tests for multiple-baseline designs. Behavioral Assessment, 8, 135-143.

“Fascinating” Issue to Contemplate This Week I will argue that the previously illustrated Marascuilo and Busk (1988) replicated AB design, with randomly determined intervention start points for each case, should be considered very nearly equivalent (in terms of its scientific credibility) to the just-presented multiple-baseline design, with random assignment of cases to the different multiple-baseline positions.

Other Randomized Start-Point Possibilities and Combinations Random Assignment of Interventions (Between Cases, Intervention Orders (Within Cases), and/or Intervention Start Points when multiple interventions are included in the study Note: The designs in the following three slides incorporate both intervention randomization and start-point randomization.

Multiple-Baseline Design With 4 Randomized Cases (“Units”), Two Within-Series Conditions, 15 Time Periods, 3, 3, 2, and 2 Potential Intervention Start Points for Cases 1, 3, 2, and 4, Respectively, and a Staggered Intervention Introduction of at Least One Time Period

Levin & Wampold’s (1999) Simultaneous Start-Point Model Time Period 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 Pair 1X A A A A A A A A A A A B* B B B B B B B B Pair 1Y A A A A A A A A A A A B* B B B B B B B B Note: X and Y are two different intervention conditions, randomly assigned to one of which is assigned to a pair member. Potential intervention start points are between Time Periods 5 and 17 inclusive. *Randomly selected intervention start point for the pair of units Levin, J. R., & Wampold, B. E. (1999). Generalized single-case randomization tests: Flexible analyses for a variety of situations. School Psychology Quarterly, 14, 59–93.

Levin & Wampold’s (1999) Replicated Simultaneous Start-Point Model Time Period 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 Pair 1X A A A A A A A A A A A B* B B B B B B B B Pair 1Y A A A A A A A A A A A B* B B B B B B B B Pair 2X A A A A A A A A A B* B B B B B B B B B B Pair 2Y A A A A A A A A A B* B B B B B B B B B B Note: X and Y are two different intervention conditions, one of which is randomly assigned to a pair member in each pair. Potential intervention start points are between Time Periods 5 and 17 inclusive. *Randomly selected intervention start point for each pair of units

In Addition… Two interesting adaptations of the preceding design: 1. One can incorporate a mixture of a randomized component (start-point randomization) and a nonrandomized component (pair member classifications) to address either individual or group interaction/moderation hypotheses. In that situation, the X and Y members of each pair would represent different variable classifications of interest (e.g., gender, developmental level, classroom achievement).

In Addition… 2. X and Y could be two different outcome measures, associated either with a single intervention or with two different interventions. For example, X could be a reading performance measure and Y an arithmetic performance measure. With a single intervention (e.g., a reading intervention), one could test the hypothesis that the intervention has comparable effects on the two measures. With two different interventions (e.g., a reading intervention and a math intervention), one could test the hypothesis that the two interventions have comparable effects on their respective outcome measures. (This particular application is illustrated in a later session.) .

Extension of Marascuilo & Busk’s (1988) Replicated AB Design Single-case analog to the conventional group crossover design (to be discussed later in the Institute): Suppose that A and B consist of two different interventions that all cases are to receive. Cases would be assigned randomly to one of the two intervention orders (AB or BA) and each case would receive a randomly selected intervention start point. So, with N = 2 cases and 20 observations, with an a priori specification of at least 7 observations for each intervention: Case 1 B B B B B B B B │A A A A A A A A A A A A Case 2 A A A A A A A A A A A A │B B B B B B B B One can test the hypothesis that the two interventions are equally effective – come back soon to see how, along with other proposed new comparative treatment designs!

Take-Home Message (to be Returned to Later in this Institute) Through various randomization schemes, it is possible to design single-case intervention studies that possess the same or similar scientific credibility characteristics as those of conventional randomized group intervention studies. With the additional inclusion of a sufficient number of replication components, who’s to say that a superbly implemented randomized single-case intervention study is less “valued” than a superbly implemented conventional randomized group intervention study? …only our Swami knows