The Role of Multisensory Information in Infant Attention to Faces of Speakers Versus the Rhythm of Speech Lorraine E. Bahrick, Mariana Vaillant-Molina,

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
CONSTRAINTS ON IMAGERY I David Pearson Room T10, William Guild Building
Advertisements

Frequency representation The ability to use the spectrum or the fine structure of sound to detect, discriminate, or identify sound.
Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination Jessica Maye, Janet F. Werker, LouAnn Gerken A brief article from Cognition.
Love and School: Attachment/Exploration Dynamics in College Jeffery E. Aspelmeier Radford University Department of Psychology Introduction Attachment theory.
INFANTS’ PERCEPTION OF FACE-AFFECT RELATIONS IN MULTIMODAL EVENTS Melissa A. Shuman & Lorraine E. Bahrick Florida International University Introduction.
Ling 240: Language and Mind Acquisition of Phonology.
T HE EFFECT OF E THNICITY ON V ISUAL L ANGUAGE D ISCRIMINATION Cathy Lin 4 th year directed study student in Psychology and Linguistics With Drs. Janet.
Development of Speech Perception. Issues in the development of speech perception Are the mechanisms peculiar to speech perception evident in young infants?
Chapter 4: Physical Development: Body, Brain, and Perception Perceptual Development By Kati Tumaneng (for Drs. Cook & Cook)
Figure 1 Mean Visual Recovery (and SD) to a novel object for trials where the object was used correctly vs. incorrectly in a moving and static display.
Effects of Sound and Visual Congruency on Product Selection and Preference Brock Bass Felipe Fernandez Drew Link Andrew Schmitz.
Chapter 6 Perception.
5-Month-Old Infants Match Facial and Vocal Expressions of Other Infants Mariana Vaillant and Lorraine E. Bahrick Florida International University Abstract.
Perception of possessions and personality traits among urban children Kara Chan, Professor Fan Hu, Ph.D. student School of Communication Hong Kong Baptist.
Research with Infants PSY 415. General Issues Sampling –Time-consuming –Expensive –Not representative? Attrition –Fussiness –Drowsiness/sleep.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS REFERENCES -Are view-boundaries special – eliciting extrapolation? There are other boundaries within a view that have similar characteristics,
NeuroPerKog: development of phonematic hearing & working memory in infants & children Włodzisław Duch & many good brains from: 1. Nicolaus Copernicus University,
Level 1 and Level 2 Auditory Perspective-taking in 3- and 4- Year -Olds Abstract Presented at the Psychology Undergraduate Research Conference, Atlanta,
Preschool-Age Sound- Shape Correspondences to the Bouba-Kiki Effect Karlee Jones, B.S. Ed. & Matthew Carter, Ph.D. Valdosta State University.
Abstract The current literature on children’s recognition of faces typically uses static faces as opposed to dynamic, moving faces (e.g., Brace, Hole,
Discussion The Effect of Auditory Sensory Abnormalities on Language Development in Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder  Autism Spectrum Disorder.
Year Review Nancy Rader May 13, esearch Emotion and Working Memory Temperament Infant Perception Attention and Early Language.
Chapter 6 Perception.
Fundamentals of Lifespan Development SEPTEMBER 19 TH, 2014 – COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT IN INFANCY AND TODDLERHOOD.
CHILD PSYCHOLOGY Canadian Edition Prepared by: Kim O'Neil, Carleton University Vasta, Miller, Ellis, Younger, Gosselin Prepared by: Kim O'Neil, Carleton.
Infancy Learning Capacities & Perceptual Development “The infant’s world is a great blooming, buzzing confusion.” ~ William James.
 The origin of grammatical rules is ascribed to an innate system in the human brain.  The knowledge of and competence for human language is acquired.
Infant Discrimination of Voices: Predictions from the Intersensory Redundancy Hypothesis Lorraine E. Bahrick, Robert Lickliter, Melissa A. Shuman, Laura.
Designing for Attention With Sound: Challenges and Extensions to Ecological Interface Design Marcus O. Watson and Penelope M. Sanderson HUMAN FACTORS,
Audiovisual Temporal Synchrony Directs Selective Listening in Four-Month-Old Infants Lorraine E. Bahrick, Melissa A. Shuman, & Irina Castellanos Department.
1. Background Evidence of phonetic perception during the first year of life: from language-universal listeners to native listeners: Consonants and vowels:
Social Orienting Impairment in Autism: Relations Among Look Length, Disengagement, and Symptom Severity Lorraine E. Bahrick, James T. Todd, Mariana Vaillant-Molina,
Three-month-old Infants Recognize Faces in Unimodal Visual but not Bimodal Audiovisual Stimulation Lorraine E. Bahrick 1, Lisa C. Newell 2, Melissa Shuman.
The role of spatial abilities and age in performance in an auditory computer navigation task Presenter: Yu-Chu Chen Adviser: Ming-Puu Chen Date: June 8,
WestEd.org Infant & Toddler Group Care Supporting Perceptual and Motor Development.
Infants’ Discrimination of Speech and Faces: Testing the Predictions of the Intersensory Redundacy Hypothesis Mariana C. Wehrhahn and Lorraine E. Bahrick.
Electrophysiological Processing of Single Words in Toddlers and School-Age Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Sharon Coffey-Corina 1, Denise Padden.
Intersensory Perception and Attention Disengagement in Young Children with Autism Lisa C. Newell 2, Lorraine E. Bahrick 1, Mariana Vaillant-Molina 1, Melissa.
Cognitive Milestones From Birth to 1 year old.
Introduction Processing of configurational information is often highly affected by inversion Previous research has focused on the perception of static.
The Development of Basic Indices of Attention to Bimodal and Unimodal Social Events Across Infancy Lorraine E. Bahrick, Barbara M. Sorondo, Irina Castellanos,
1 Cross-language evidence for three factors in speech perception Sandra Anacleto uOttawa.
CSD 2230 INTRODUCTION TO HUMAN COMMUNICATION DISORDERS Normal Sound Perception, Speech Perception, and Auditory Characteristics at the Boundaries of the.
Abstract Bahrick and Lickliter (2000) recently proposed an intersensory redundancy hypothesis that states that early in development information presented.
Basic cognitive processes - 1 Psych 414 Prof. Jessica Sommerville.
Basic Cognitive Processes - 2
Abstract Prior research has demonstrated that young infants are able to perceive the affordance, or the potential for action, provided by the physical.
Bahrick, L. E. (1995). Intermodal origins of self-perception. In P. Rochat (Ed.), The self in infancy: Theory and research. Advances in Psychology Series.
Infant Perception of Object-Affect Relations Mariana Vaillant-Molina and Lorraine E. Bahrick Florida International University Presented at the Society.
The Role of Intersensory Redundancy in the Typical Development of Social Orienting Across Infancy: A New Hypothesis for Autism Lorraine E. Bahrick, James.
Development of Basic Indices of Attention to Nonsocial Events Across Infancy: Effects of Unimodal versus Bimodal Stimulation Lorraine E. Bahrick, James.
The Development of Face Perception in Dynamic, Multimodal Events: Predictions from the Intersensory Redundancy Hypothesis Lorraine E. Bahrick, Robert Lickliter,
Infants’ Detection of the Affordances of Everyday Objects Katryna Anasagasti, Lorraine E. Bahrick, and Laura C. Batista Florida International University.
Perception By: Sandra Rodriguez  Perception creates an object from sensory stimulation.  Our perceptual processes determine that certain features go.
Intersensory Redundancy Facilitates Infants’ Perception of Meaning in Speech Passages Irina Castellanos, Melissa Shuman, and Lorraine E. Bahrick Florida.
The Development of Infants’ Sensitivity to the Orientation of Object Motion: Predictions of the Intersensory Redundancy Hypothesis Lorraine E. Bahrick.
Research Paper: The role of sensory processing in word learning in children at risk of autism By Tatjana Zeljic, MSc Ed. Neuroscience.
& Results: Parenting & Line Judgments ► Parents’ autonomy scores are significantly.
Methods Identifying the Costs of Auditory Dominance on Visual Processing: An Eye Tracking Study Wesley R. Barnhart, Samuel Rivera, & Christopher W. Robinson.
THE EFFECT OF PRENATAL SENSORY STIMULATION ON PERCEPTUAL NARROWING IN BOBWHITE QUAIL NEONATES Briana O’Dowd.
Adults’ Gestures During Object Naming and Preverbal Infants’ Word Mapping: Does Type of Motion Matter? Lakshmi J. Gogate & Dalit J. Matatyaho State University.
Susan Geffen, Suzanne Curtin and Susan Graham
Susan Geffen, Suzanne Curtin and Susan Graham
in Single and Multiple Modalities,
The Development of Emotional Interactions Across the Senses:
CEN3722 Human Computer Interaction Displays
Introduction to Sensation and Perception
47th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Houston, TX
Title of Project School Name Miami, FL Introduction Problem Statement
Multisensory Processes: A Balancing Act across the Lifespan
Presentation transcript:

The Role of Multisensory Information in Infant Attention to Faces of Speakers Versus the Rhythm of Speech Lorraine E. Bahrick, Mariana Vaillant-Molina, Melissa A. Shuman, Irina Castellanos, Laura C. Batista and Mariana Werhahn Florida International University Abstract According to the intersensory redundancy hypothesis (IRH), information presented redundantly and synchronously across two or more senses recruits selective attention and facilitates perceptual learning of amodal properties (e.g., rhythm, tempo, synchrony) more successfully than information that is presented to only one sense. In contrast, information available to only one sense such as the face of a silent person or a voice from a nearby room, facilitates perception of modality specific properties such as facial configuration or timbre and pitch of the voice (Bahrick & Lickliter, 2000; Bahrick, Flom, & Lickliter, 2002). Previous research has found that at the youngest ages tested, infants can detect rhythm and tempo changes in nonsocial events in bimodal, audiovisual presentations, but not in unimodal (audio or visual) presentations (Bahrick & Lickliter, 2000; Bahrick, Flom, & Lickliter, 2002). Only after a few months of additional experience with multimodal stimulation, can infants detect rhythm and tempo in both unimodal and bimodal presentations (Bahrick & Lickliter, 2004). In this study we extended the above research to the domain of social events. We assessed whether infant sensitivity to the rhythm of speech would be facilitated in bimodal, redundant audiovisual presentations and attenuated in unimodal visual presentations, and in contrast, infant discrimination of the face of the speaker would be facilitated in unimodal visual and attenuated in bimodal, audiovisual presentations. Thirty-two 2.5-month-old infants were habituated, in an infant control procedure, to a video of one of two women speaking a nursery rhyme in one of two distinctive rhythms. They received two types of test trials, one depicting a change in the rhythm of speech and the other, a change in the face of the speaker. Visual recovery to the novel stimulus was measured. Results support the IRH and indicate that infants detect the change in the rhythm of speech only when the presentation is bimodal (audiovisual), whereas they detect the change of face only when the presentation is unimodal. These findings suggest that during social interaction, such as observing a person speaking, selective attention of the infant shifts from features of the face to temporal properties of the speech as a function of whether the visible person is speaking or silent. These results further highlight the important role of intersensory redundancy in guiding and constraining selective attention in early development. Introduction Infants spend much of their time in close face-to-face interactions which provide a range of multimodal and unimodal stimulation including the configuration of facial features, the pitch and timbre of the voice, and the rhythm and tempo of speech. According to the intersensory redundancy hypothesis (IRH), information presented redundantly and synchronously across two or more senses recruits selective attention and facilitates perceptual learning of amodal properties (e.g., rhythm, tempo, synchrony) more successfully than information that is presented to only one sense. In contrast, information available to only one sense such as the face of a silent person or a voice from a nearby room, facilitates perception of modality specific properties such as facial configuration or timbre and pitch of the voice (Bahrick & Lickliter, 2000; Bahrick, Flom, & Lickliter, 2002). Previous research has found that at the youngest ages tested, infants can detect rhythm and tempo changes in nonsocial events (such as a toy hammer tapping) in bimodal, audiovisual presentations, but not in unimodal (auditory or visual) presentations (Bahrick & Lickliter, 2000; Bahrick, Flom, & Lickliter, 2002). Only older infants who have had a few months of additional experience with multimodal stimulation can detect rhythm and tempo in both unimodal and bimodal presentations (Bahrick & Lickliter, 2004). In contrast, research on detection of modality specific information has shown that young infants detect a change in the face of a speaker or the orientation of a moving object in unimodal visual stimulation but not in bimodal audiovisual stimulation (Bahrick, Lickliter, Vaillant, Shuman, & Castellanos, 2004). The present study extended our research on perception of rhythm to the domain of social events (audiovisual speech), included tests for detection of both amodal information (rhythm of speech) and modality specific information (facial configuration) in the same study and for the same infants, and tested younger infants (2.5 month olds) than in our prior studies of nonsocial events. Following habituation to videos of a woman speaking, in bimodal speech, infants were expected to discriminate a change in the rhythm of speech but not the face of the speaker, whereas in unimodal visual speech infants were expected to discriminate a change in the face of the speaker but not the rhythm of speech, following predictions of the IRH. Method Thirty-two 2.5 month old infants were habituated, in an infant controlled procedure, to a video of one of two women (see Figure 1) speaking a nursery rhyme in one of two distinctive rhythms. Sixteen infants received bimodal audiovisual displays of the woman speaking and sixteen infants received unimodal visual displays of the woman speaking silently throughout habituation and test. Following habituation (a decrease in looking time of 50%), each infant received two types of test trials under their respective conditions. One test trial depicted a change in the rhythm of speech (with no other changes) and the other depicted a change in the woman’s face (with no other changes). Visual recovery to the changes (with respect to two no-change posthabituation trials) served as the measure of discrimination. Results Results (see Figure 2) support our hypotheses and demonstrate that infants in the bimodal audiovisual condition showed significant visual recovery to the change in the rhythm of speech (t(15)=3.64, p=.002 ) but not to the change in the face of the speaker (t(15)= 1.63, p>.1). In contrast, in the unimodal visual condition infants showed the reverse pattern. They demonstrated significant visual recovery to the change in the face of the speaker (t(15)=2.42, p=.031), but not to the change in the rhythm of speech (t(15)=.984, p>.1). Conclusions These findings support predictions of the IRH and demonstrate that in bimodal, redundant stimulation (audiovisual speech), infants were able to discriminate a change in amodal information (rhythm of speech) but not modality specific information (facial configuration). In contrast, in unimodal visual stimulation (silent visual speech), infants were able to discriminate a change in modality specific information (facial configuration) but not amodal information (rhythm of speech). These findings suggest that during social interactions, such as observing a person speaking, selective attention of the infant shifts from features of the face to temporal properties of the speech as a function of whether the visible person is speaking or silent. These results further highlight the important role of intersensory redundancy in guiding and constraining selective attention in early development. References Bahrick, L.E., & Lickliter, R. (2000). Intersensory redundancy guides attentional selectivity and perceptual learning in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 36, Bahrick, LE., Flom, R., & Lickliter, R. (2002). Intersensory redundancy facilitates discrimination of tempo in 3-month-old infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 41, Bahrick, L.E., & Lickliter, R. (2004). Infants' perception of rhythm and tempo in unimodal and multimodal stimulation: A developmental test of the intersensory redundancy hypothesis. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, Bahrick, L. E., Lickliter, R., Vaillant, M., Shuman, M., & Castellanos, I. (2004, June). The development of face perception in dynamic, multimodal events: Predictions from the intersensory redundancy hypothesis. Fifth Annual Meeting of the International Multisensory Research Forum, Barcelona, Spain. Presented at the Society for Research in Child Development Biennial Meeting, April, 2005, Atlanta, GA. This research was supported by NIMH grant RO1 MH to the first author. Requests for reprints should be sent to the first author at Bimodal Unimodal Audiovisual Visual ** p >.01 * p >.05 Figure 1 Figure 2. Mean visual recovery (and SD) to a change in rhythm of speech and face of speaker Visual Recovery (seconds)