Dynamics in using different question types in Estonian police interviews of children Kristjan Kask, PhD University Nord, Estonia.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Recorded conversations between parents and children when suspecting child sexual abuse Iiirg 2011, Dundee Julia Korkman, PhD Helsinki University Hospital.
Advertisements

The Effects of Age and Negative Feedback on Witness Response iIIRG Conference Toronto, Canada 24 – 26 May 2012 Dr. Hazel McMurtrie
Criminological Psychology Charlton et al 2000: St. Helena study.
Vulnerable children and their right to be heard Ann-Christin Cederborg Professor and Head of the department: Child and Youth Studies, Stockholm university.
WorkSafe Victoria is a division of the Victorian WorkCover Authority Guidance Note on the Prevention of Bullying and Violence at Work Evaluation results.
Revising the context reinstatement component of the Cognitive Interview for older eyewitnesses. Rachel Wilcock 1 & Coral Dando 2 1 London South Bank University.
“Did She Do it on Purpose?” Young Children’s Accuracy for Questions About Intentions “Did She Do it on Purpose?” Young Children’s Accuracy for Questions.
Eye-witness testimony
The Forensic Interviewer at Trial Strategies for Defending the Interview in Court.
Learning outcome: By the end of this 25 minutes you will be able to discuss a strength and a limitation of using qualitative methods to study children’s.
Young people, money and access to tobacco Grace Wong, Marewa Glover, Vili Nosa, Becky Freeman, Janine Paynter, Robert Scragg.
Child interviews in legal settings Policy and practise in Finland Julia Korkman, PhD Helsinki University Hospital & Åbo Akademi University.
Interviewing and Testimony
Repeated questions and repeated interviews in forensic investigations with intellectually disabled victims David La Rooy University of.
It’s all to play for: an alternative approach to rapport building in child investigative interviewing Kimberly Collins and Dr Martin Doherty, Dept of Psychology,
Child Investigative Interviewing: Everything You Need To know
THE COGNITIVE INTERVIEW Improving Eye Witness Testimony.
Overparenting: A Cross-Cultural Analysis
The Cognitive Interview
Makiko NAKA 1, Yukinori FUTAKUCHI2 and Toshikazu KOYAMA 2 1 Hokkaido University, 2 Hokkaido Child Guidance Center Key words: Investigative interviewing,
Eyewitness Identification Interviewing By: Matt Sullivan.
Physical Development in Adolescent Children A Timeline of Milestones in Children Ages 8-11 Years.
Presented by Louise & charlotte.. Psychologists over the years have been trying to develop memory retrieval techniques aimed at trying to entice more.
Child Protection Policies Training Prepared by: SUNY Office of General Counsel SUNY Compliance Office 2015.
Assisting evidential presentation using sketch plans Kerry Marlow and Martyn Hilbourne 2010.
Father Involvement and Child Well-Being: 2006 Survey of Income and Program Participation (SIPP) Child Well-Being Topical Module 1 By Jane Lawler Dye Fertility.
Child interviewing in Scotland Thank you Deirdre Brown, Sonja Brubacher, Ann-Christin Cederborg, Coral Dando, Fiona Gabbert, John Halley, Carmit Katz,
Thorbjorg Sveinsdottir - Children´s house Multi-agency and Interdisciplinary Approach to Child Sexual Abuse.
AQA Questions and answers
Kirstie Pye, Research Officer NWORTH Clinical Trials Unit Bangor University.
Training of police officers in Sweden -the development of interview behaviour Professor Ann-Christin Cederborg Department of Child and Youth Studies, Stockholm.
CHILE 1 Adaptation process of an evidence based model of investigative interviewing: the Chilean case* Carolina Navarro, Decio Mettifogo, Andrés.
Incarcerated Fathers Skype with Their Children from Prison: Outcome Indicators of Children’s Well-Being Kerry Kazura, Ph.D. Human Development and Family.
Mothers’ Questions During Personal Narrative and Book Reading Conversations Chavelis Rosario Mentor: Gigliana Melzi, Ph.D. New York University.
Experience Sharing and Learning from Adolescents Development Programmes Youth Helpline-Rozan.
Is the cognitive interview efficient on very young children's ability to testify about an occurrence of a repeated event? Fanny Verkampt, Cindy Colomb,
Week 3 Graham Davies Child Witnesses and the Courtroom.
BDI Training. Agenda General Information about BDI Administering the BDI Scoring the BDI Watch BDI on 2 year old – score protocol.
General EAP writing instruction and transfer of learning Mark Andrew James Arizona State University
* Do parent’s dominate hand determine their child’s dominate hand?
2007 Family Talk About Drinking Study. Introduction The 2007 Family Talk Study was conducted by Data Development Worldwide among those individuals who.
Memorise these words, you have until I have finished reading them out. sournicecandy honeysugarsoda bitterchocolategood hearttastecake toothtartpie.
 Social cognitive theory is acquiring symbolic representations through observation.  Learning through imitation of observed behaviour.
Acknowledgments We thank Dr. Yu, Dr. Bateman, and Professor Szabo for allowing us to conduct this study during their class time. We especially thank the.
Four-year trends in sport participation and retention: the gender differences R.M Eime 1,2., J.T Harvey 1., M.J Charity 1,2., M.M Casey 1., H Westerbeek.
 By preschool age, boys and girls show marked differences on a number of emotional, social, and behavioral outcomes (Ruble et al., 2006). Some gender.
Do Japanese children say ‘yes’ to their mothers? A naturalistic study of response bias in parent- toddler conversations Mako Okanda (Kyoto University)
Police officers’ perceptions of the Cognitive Interview: Usefulness, confidence and witness reliability. Dr Jacqueline Wheatcroft (University of Liverpool)
The Cognitive Interview – Fisher and Geiselman (1995) Can I describe the various techniques used in the cognitive interview? Can I describe research studies.
The Role of Close Family Relationships in Predicting Multisystemic Therapy Outcome: An Investigation of Sex Differences ABSTRACT BACKGROUND: Multisystemic.
Results Time 2 (Age 18-20) Target teen and their romantic partner engaged in an 8 minute hypothetical disagreement task interaction. Hostile, relationship-undermining.
1 The NICHD Protocol: A best practice to conduct investigative interviews with children Mireille Cyr, Ph.D. Université de Montréal Knowledge Exchange organized.
Repeated questions in police interviews with young children alleging abuse Sarah Krähenbühl.
Samuel & Bryant Developmental Psychology The Core Studies.
Discussion Results Introduction From Lateral to Leader: A Study of Preschoolers’ Relationships with Peers Erin Podgorski & Dr. Carin L. Neitzel, The University.
Faculty Adviser: Dr. Deborah Elledge  Student Researchers: Leah Carpenter – Jacqueline Oakes – Jillian Utz Communication Sciences and Disorders Department.
Adherence to Achieving Best Evidence Guidelines in Child Forensic Interviews: A Survey of Police Officers in England Eleanor Coles MSc Forensic Psychology.
Questioning Children Rita Farrell, Forensic Interview Specialist
Cognitive interview.
An examination of police recruits’ planning and interviewing practices
The Use of Anatomical Dolls
Galia Ran & Rachel Gali Cinamon Career Development Lab,
Cognitive Interviews Eyewitness Testimony.
Starter Imagine - you did not do as well as you wanted to in a biology test, but your teacher praises you for working hard and trying your best. You feel.
PSYA1: Cognitive Psychology Memory
International Evidence-Based Investigative Interviewing of Children
Interviewing witnesses
Ministry of Social Security and Labour
The Bugs Bunny Effect
The cognitive area.
Presentation transcript:

Dynamics in using different question types in Estonian police interviews of children Kristjan Kask, PhD University Nord, Estonia

Question types Open memory prompts tend to elicit longer and more accurate responses than do closed prompts (Hershkowitz, Lamb, Sternberg, & Esplin, 1997; Lamb & Fauchier, 2001; Lamb, Hershkowitz, Sternberg, Esplin, et al., 1996; Orbach & Lamb, 1999). Focused and closed questioning has been found to be dominant in forensic interviews of children in Israel, Sweden, Finland, the United States, and the UK (Cederborg, Orbach, Sternberg, & Lamb, 2000; Craig, Scheibe, Kircher, Raskin, & Dodd, 1999; Davies, Westcott, & Horan, 2000; Lamb, Hershkowitz, Sternberg, Boat, & Everson, 1996; Sternberg et al., 1996; Sternberg, Lamb, Davies, & Westcott, 2001; Stockdale, 1996; Walker & Hunt, 1998; Santtila, Korkela, & Häkkänen, 2004; Korkman, Santtila, & Sandnabba, 2006).

Children’s free recall Young children’s free recall is accurate if they are interviewed appropriately (Flin, Boon, Knox, & Bull, 1992; Marin et al., 1979). When children are allowed to recall information freely or through the use of general questions, even very young children can produce material which is highly accurate (Hutcheson, Baxter, Telfer, & Warden, 1995).

Use of questions In forensic contexts, free recall prompts such as general or cued invitations produce three to five times more information than do focused prompts (Lamb, Hershkowitz, Sternberg, Esplin, et al., 1996; Sternberg et al., 1996; Sternberg, Lamb, Davies, & Westcott, 2001). Option-posing, ´yes/no´, and suggestive interviewer utterances are more likely to elicit inaccurate information because they implicitly encourage children to recall the suggested information or to guess (Dent & Stephenson, 1979; Poole & White, 1993; Ceci & Bruck, 1995; Poole & Lindsay, 1998).

Present study The purpose of the present study was to examine the questioning styles employed in Estonian police interviews of child sexual and physical abuse cases, focusing  (i) on the proportions of questions and number of words in children’s answers to the questions, and  (ii) the dynamics in using questions during the interviews (see also Griffiths Question Map, Griffiths & Milne, 2006).

Method Participants Videotaped interviews with 66 children (23 boys and 43 girls), mean age was 9 years 6 months (range 5 to 13 years). The interviews had been conducted during 2004/5 and 2007/8, mean length 27 minutes (range 8 to 82 minutes). Sixty three (95%) of the interviewers were female police officers and three (5%) were male.

Method No information was available concerning the officers’ experience, the outcome of the interviews, or what proportion led to trial or conviction. In general, Estonian police officers have had training about interviewing suspects and witnesses during their general studies, and specific trainings in interviewing children (such as the introduction to the Cognitive Interview or NICHD protocol) but these occur very infrequently and are not necessarily part of involving new investigators’ into the teams.

Coding Videotapes of the interviews were transcribed. The categories by Lamb and colleagues (Lamb, Hershkowitz, Sternberg, Esplin et al., 1996; Lamb, Hershkowitz, Sternberg, Boat, & Everson, 1996) and by Yuille and Cutshall (1986) were used to characterize the interviewer utterances and to measure the amount of new information provided.

Coding Explanations Invitations:  General  Cued Directive utterances Option-posing utterances Suggestive utterances Verbal affirmations / facilitators

Coding The number of words in the children’s responses was counted in all interviews in response on interviewers’ questions. Each interview was divided into four equal quarters regardless of how many questions were asked to investigate the proportionate usage of different question types across time within interviews.

Results The interviewers on average asked more than one question per utterance M = 1.37 (0.60) range from 1 to 9. The average number of questions asked per interview was M = 246 (164), range from 44 to 841) and the average number of words reported by children was M = 823 (671), range from 63 to 2641.

Total number of words in children’s responses The total number of words in children’s responses increased with age F(1,2) = 5.39, p <.01, η² =.15 M = (553.8) 4 to 7-year-olds M = (609.9) 8 to 11-year-olds M = (721.1) 12 to 14-year olds

Question types Option-posing (32%) and direct questions (32%) were used mostly by the interviewers followed by verbal affirmations (21.6%), explanations (7.6%), suggestive questions (4.1%), cued invitations (1.6%), and general invitations (1.1%).

The number of words in response to different categories of interviewer utterance (F (1,6) = 87.46, p <.001, η² =.049) CategoryMeanSD Explanation General invitation Cued invitation Verbal affirmation Direct questions Option-posing questions Suggestive questions Mean

Average number of words in children’s responses The average number of words in children’s responses increased with age F(1,2) = , p <.001, η² =.041. M = 2.69 (3.28) 4 to 7-year-olds M = 5.22 (6.13) 8 to 11-year-olds M = 6.41 (6.78) 12 to 14-year olds

The proportion of questions in different quarters of interviews (number of questions) Category First quarter Second quarter Third quarter Forth quarter Chi-sq. Explanation p <.001 General invitation p <.001 Cued invitation p <.001 Verbal affirmation ns Direct questions p <.001 Option- posing questions p <.001 Suggestive questions p <.001

Conclusion The continuing failure to encourage a free narrative (or to try and maintain it once the child had begun) with many of the children is a concern, since this allows children to present their ´story` in their own words (see Westcott & Kynan, 2006). One possible explanation for the overall large number of option-posing questions may be that officers are not aware of the negative effect of such questions. Police officers may feel pressured by legal demands associated with investigative interviews on the one hand and the psychological well-being of the child during the questioning on the other hand.

Conclusion Better police training in the adoption of best- practice guidelines in interviewing children is required. Methods such as the NICHD protocol or cognitive interview are strongly recommended to structure the interviews.

Thank you!