BULLYING AND BELONGING: A LONGITUDINAL STUDY OF THE PROTECTIVE ROLE OF DEFENDERS IN FRIENDSHIP GROUPS Siân Jones, Claire Fox, Simon Hunter, & Jon
GROUPS AND BULLYING
Within 51 cases reported by teachers, there were 23 cases where a single target was bullied by a group of children, but the target was then supported by other children. Children (friends of the bullied) approached me and told me about what had happened, giving me names of the bullies, also of other children who could corroborate their story.[....] they had not approached any other teachers or informed their parents (P 19, years) I discovered that a group of girls in my class were bullying one particular child... there were about 7 or 8 involved altogether (P30, years). GROUPS AND BULLYING Jones et al., in prep.
DEFENDERS Defenders are those children who “take sides with the victims, comforting and supporting them” (Salmivalli, 2010, p. 114). Defenders tend to: be emotionally stable (Tani, Greenman, Schneider, & Fregoso, 2003) be cognitively skilled (Caravita, DiBlasio, & Salmivalli, 2009). be empathic (e.g., Caravita et al., 2009) have high self-efficacy in their defending ability, (Thornberg & Jungert, 2013). Pozzoli and Gini (2010) found high levels of defending behaviour under conditions of high perceived peer pressure, even when personal responsibility for intervening was low. Defenders are those children who “take sides with the victims, comforting and supporting them” (Salmivalli, 2010, p. 114). Defenders tend to: be emotionally stable (Tani, Greenman, Schneider, & Fregoso, 2003) be cognitively skilled (Caravita, DiBlasio, & Salmivalli, 2009). be empathic (e.g., Caravita et al., 2009) have high self-efficacy in their defending ability, (Thornberg & Jungert, 2013). Pozzoli and Gini (2010) found high levels of defending behaviour under conditions of high perceived peer pressure, even when personal responsibility for intervening was low.
EFFECTS OF DEFENDERS Salmivalli, Voeten, and Poskiparta (2011) showed that defending the victim was negatively associated with the frequency of bullying in a classroom. Sainio et al. (2011) found that being defended was positively related to victims’ adjustment and social status. What about the effect of defenders over time on bullying at the peer (friendship) group level? Salmivalli, Voeten, and Poskiparta (2011) showed that defending the victim was negatively associated with the frequency of bullying in a classroom. Sainio et al. (2011) found that being defended was positively related to victims’ adjustment and social status. What about the effect of defenders over time on bullying at the peer (friendship) group level?
CHILDREN’S FRIENDSHIPS Friendships are generally considered to be protective against peer victimization. However, the sheer number of friends, and simply having a very best-friend may not be sufficient in protecting children (Hodges & Perry, 1999). Fox and Boulton (2006) found that the number of friends, and the peer acceptance of a very best-friend attenuate experiences of victimization over time. The identity of children’s friends (i.e. those who are not weak or victimised themselves) and the quality of the friendship (i.e. how much a child ‘sticks up for their friend’) are more important (Hodges et al., 1999).
INGROUP IDENTIFICATION Ingroup identification has a moderating influence on children’s reactions to intergroup events. Nesdale, Durkin, Maass, and Griffiths (2005) found that children’s ethnic prejudice was positively related to strength of identification with their ethnic ingroup. Jones, Manstead, and Livingstone (2009, 2011) showed that group-based reactions to bullying intensified as a function of group membership and in-group identification. Levels of identification with the friendship group therefore influence group members’ willingness to stick with and support the group.
BULLYING AND SOCIAL IDENTITY Social network research on bullying has shown that defenders are well-liked (Salmivalli et al., 1996) and popular among their peers (Caravita et al., 2009).
Sample of UK children, aged years (M = years, SD = 0.64 years, 612 male, 93% white). Data were collected at two time points – Autumn and Summer Terms. Children completed peer nominations of peer victimization (e.g., Björkqvist, Lagerspetz, & Kaukiainen, 1992). Sample of UK children, aged years (M = years, SD = 0.64 years, 612 male, 93% white). Data were collected at two time points – Autumn and Summer Terms. Children completed peer nominations of peer victimization (e.g., Björkqvist, Lagerspetz, & Kaukiainen, 1992). ESRC SCHOOL BULLYING PROJECT
Children were asked to nominate a best friend, and their friends, in the class. They were asked to give each classmate a rating from 1 “dislike very much”, to 5 “like very much”. Children were asked to nominate a best friend, and their friends, in the class. They were asked to give each classmate a rating from 1 “dislike very much”, to 5 “like very much”. ESRC SCHOOL BULLYING PROJECT
IDENTIFYING FRIENDSHIP GROUPS Based on Baines & Blatchford (2009). A group is defined as a set of children (N ≥2) each of whom reciprocally nominates at least two others (or one other, where N=2) in the group as a friend or best friend, and reciprocally gives at least one of those a friendship rating of 5 (“like very much”). 11
HYPOTHESES We hypothesized that having defenders in the friendship group, and having children with multiple friendship group associations in the friendship group, would be negatively related over time to the levels of peer victimization. We controlled for the effects of gender, peer acceptance, number of membership groups, and class size. Peer Victimization at Time 1 Peer Victimization at Time 2 Defenders in Friendship Groups Multiple Group Associations of Friends
LONGITUDINAL DATA ANALYSES Adjusted R 2 =.33, ß=-.155, t =-3.86, p<.001. Hierarchical Linear Regression Effects of defenders and networked-ness on later victimization
IMPLICATIONS The reduction in peer victimization as a function of the number of defenders and ‘networked’ children in a child’s friendship group emphasizes the need to investigate bullying as a group phenomenon at the level of the friendship group in real friendship groups. How the effect of group-identification might vary depending on group norms could also be examined, given Polozzi and Gini’s (2010) finding that perceived peer group pressure predicted defending behaviour even when personal responsibility for intervening was low.
PRACTICAL IMPLICATIONS An explicit focus on friendship groups would be a valuable addition to existing anti-bullying strategies. Encouraging and enabling children to defend and support victimized peers can help reduce levels of victimization. Our results support the importance of friendship groups within schools and emphasise the need to network individuals across those friendship groups to ensure they can have maximum impact.
SUMMARY We aimed to explore the effect of having defenders in one’s friendship group, and having networked friends in one’s friendship group, on peer victimization. The longitudinal design allowed us to look at the cause-effect relationship between defending and later levels of victimization. Previous research had not looked at the effect of defenders in friendship groups. We aimed to explore the effect of having defenders in one’s friendship group, and having networked friends in one’s friendship group, on peer victimization. The longitudinal design allowed us to look at the cause-effect relationship between defending and later levels of victimization. Previous research had not looked at the effect of defenders in friendship groups.
SUMMARY Having more defenders in one’s friendship group, and having children in one’s friendship group who are networked across multiple friendship groups, reduces peer victimization across a nine month period. We now need to better understand the social identity concerns of (a) defenders, and (b) those who belong to multiple friendship groups, as a basis for developing anti-bullying interventions encouraging intragroup defending of victims. Having more defenders in one’s friendship group, and having children in one’s friendship group who are networked across multiple friendship groups, reduces peer victimization across a nine month period. We now need to better understand the social identity concerns of (a) defenders, and (b) those who belong to multiple friendship groups, as a basis for developing anti-bullying interventions encouraging intragroup defending of victims.
Siân ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Claire Fox Simon Hunter Keele Research Team Jon Kennedy Susan Pratley All the children who took part, and the schools and parents who allowed them to do so
A CLASS NETWORK N = 17 Group 1Group 2 Group Group 4 19
CORRELATIONS Mean SD Time 1 Peer Victimization 2. Time 2 Peer Victimization.748*** 3. Time 1 % Defenders in Group.115** Time 2 % Defenders in Group.138**.150**.248*** 5. Time 1 N of Membership Groups-.212*** Time 2 N of Membership Groups-.161***-.216*** *** 7. Time 1 Peer Acceptance-.385***-.369*** ***.259*** 8. Time 2 Peer Acceptance-.096*-.123** ***.122** 9. Gender.133***.112** ** ** 10. Time 1 % of Networked Group Members in Membership Groups * Time 2 % of Networked Group Members in Membership Groups.146***.102** ** ***.097 *p <.05; **p <.01, ***p <.001.
GROUP STATS Size and Number of Friendship Groups Across Time Points