Intensity discrimination is the process of distinguishing one stimulus intensity from another Intensity Discrimination.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Psychophysics 4 Research Methods Fall 2010 Tamás Bőhm.
Advertisements

Scaling. Scaling seeks to discover how varying the physical parameters of the stimulus affects the psychological parameters. In general, scaling is concerned.
Chapter 5: Space and Form Form & Pattern Perception: Humans are second to none in processing visual form and pattern information. Our ability to see patterns.
Approaches to the Study of Perception
M. Zareinejad.  Methodology for investigating relationships between sensations in the psychological domain and stimuli in the physical domain  Central.
Thresholds, Weber’s law, Fechner’s three methods Research Methods Fall 2010 Tamás Bőhm.
We can see objects even though the background luminance levels change over a range of more than 10 orders of magnitude (10 10 ). How do we do it?
PSYCHOPHYSICS What is Psychophysics? Classical Psychophysics Thresholds Signal Detection Theory Psychophysical Laws.
Rob van der Willigen designed by Stephanie Thái.
The Perception of Correlation in Scatterplots Ronald A. Rensink Departments of Computer Science and Psychology University of British Columbia Vancouver,
Psychophysics 3 Research Methods Fall 2010 Tamás Bőhm.
Sensation & Perception
Intensity discrimination is the process of distinguishing one stimulus intensity from another Intensity Discrimination.
Click the Speaker button to listen to Exploring Psychology.
Applied Psychoacoustics Lecture 4: Loudness Jonas Braasch.
Where vision is concerned, light is generally specified in photometric units, not in quanta of light (photons) and energy.
Sensation and Perception Chapters 5 & 6. Some Basic Questions How do we sense the world?
UNIT 4: SENSATION & PERCEPTION Module 12. Sensation & Perception Sensation: the process by which you detect physical energy from your environment and.
Perceptual Issues Humans can discriminate about ½ a minute of arc –At fovea, so only in center of view, 20/20 vision –At 1m, about 0.2mm (“Dot Pitch”
The Lion King Do you see the message hidden?
The problem of contrast metric for reaction time to aperiodic stimuli Angel Vassilev 1, 2 Adrian Murzac 2, Margarita B. Zlatkova 3 & Roger S. Anderson.
© by Yu Hen Hu 1 Human Visual System. © by Yu Hen Hu 2 Understanding HVS, Why? l Image is to be SEEN! l Perceptual Based Image Processing.
.  Sensation: process by which our sensory receptors and nervous system receive and represent stimulus energy  Perception: process of organizing and.
Sensation The process by which our sensory receptors receive stimulus energies from our environment. Sensory receptors detect millions of stimuli.
Why cover “acuity” in a course called “Central Visual Mechanisms”? …because we are now talking about how the whole visual system works and how we can measure.
Jayme Shadowens.  Senses = filters  Process incoming information  Physical stimulation into neural impulses that give us sensations  Sensation: the.
Test your Awareness 1 (basketball game). Core Concepts in Understanding Sensation and Perception AP Unit 3 Reading pp
Sensation and Perception Sensory input and Psychophysics.
Serial Conscious Processing Slower than parallel processing Allows us to solve new problems which require focus Volunteers?
Weber Fractions Decibel Scale Of Sound Energy.
Sensation and Perception - psychophysics.ppt © 2001 Laura Snodgrass, Ph.D. Psychophysics Outline Classical Psychophysics –definition –psychometric function.
SENSATION AND PERCEPTION. Sensation: the stimulation of sense organs---absorption of energy (light/sound waves) Perception: selection, organization, and.
Fundamentals of Sensation and Perception EXPLORING PERCEPTION BY STUDYING BEHAVIOUR ERIK CHEVRIER SEPTEMBER 16 TH, 2015.
Dimension being tested Remained constant during testing.
VISUAL ACUITY. Visual Acuity: Is the smallest visual angle that a person can see clearly. We will talk about four very different tasks… Yet all of these.
Human Capabilities Part – I. Hearing (Chapter 6*) Prepared by: Ahmed M. El-Sherbeeny, PhD 1.
Psychophysics and Psychoacoustics
Experimental Psychology PSY 433
Sensation and Perception
Psych 480: Fundamentals of Perception and Sensation
Myers PSYCHOLOGY Seventh Edition in Modules
Sensation & Perception ATTENTION, PROCESSING, THRESHOLDS.
Response Processes Psych DeShon. Response Elicitation Completion Completion Requires production Requires production Allows for creative responses.
Measurements and Their Analysis. Introduction Note that in this chapter, we are talking about multiple measurements of the same quantity Numerical analysis.
Human Visual System.
Sensation & Perception A.P. Psychology Chapter 4.
Sensation and Perception Sensation: your window to the world Perception: interpreting what comes in your window.
Sensation and Perception. Sense: Translates outside information nervous system activity –“Sensations” Perception: Process that turns sensations into experience.
Psy Psychology of Hearing Psychophysics and Detection Theory Neal Viemeister
Signal detection Psychophysics.
Sharp Targets Are Detected Better Against a Figure, and Blurred Targets Are Detected Better Against a Background Eva Wong and Noami Weisstein, 1983.
Sensation & Perception Chapter 5. Sensation & Perception The “five” senses: – sight, hearing taste, smell, touch, vestibular & kinesthetic Sensory organs.
Sensation & Perception. An Introductory Activity Researchers have found that our experiences influence our perceptions Look at the following photographs.
PRINCIPLES OF SENSORY TRANSDUCTION
Introduction (See the Sensation & Perception, 4e Instructor’s Manual for helpful notes that accompany this presentation; these are given in a conversational.
Psychology Ch. 3 Sensation and Perception
Sensation & Perception
Human Visual System.
Sensation and Perception
Sensation & Perception
Figure Legend: From: Binocular combination of luminance profiles
Sensation and Perception
Classical psychophysics
Volume 34, Issue 5, Pages (May 2002)
Senses.
Introductory Psychology Concepts
Volume 34, Issue 5, Pages (May 2002)
Chapter 6 (B): Thresholds and Sensory Adaptation
Experiencing the World
Principles of perception
Presentation transcript:

Intensity discrimination is the process of distinguishing one stimulus intensity from another Intensity Discrimination

Two types: Difference thresholds – the two stimuli are physically separate Increment thresholds – the two stimuli are immediately adjacent or superimposed

Fig. 1.1 Difference threshold (separated stimuli) Increment threshold stimuli (edges of stimuli touch each other)

Fig. 1.2

Theory and Practice Theory:

Fig. 2.7 From Dr. Kraft’s course – Hecht, Shlaer & Pirenne Photon emission follows a Poisson distribution To distinguish a flash with a mean of 8 from a flash with a mean of 9 quanta is impossible! The distributions overlap almost completely

Mean of 8, vs. mean of 9 If present the flash with a mean of 8 photons many times and a flash with a mean of 9 photons presented many times, there will be many times that the 9 photon flash will have more photons than the 8 photon flash

Mean of 8, vs. mean of 12

Mean of 8, vs. mean of 16

Mean of 8, vs. mean of 20

In a Poisson distribution, the variance is equal to the mean. The standard deviation (SD) is the square root of the mean. In a two-alternative forced-choice task, to reach threshold (75% correct), L T must differ from L by 0.95 SD. (e.g.,  L = 0.95 SD)

Moreover, as L increases, the minimum threshold  L also increases with the because the variance in a Poisson distribution equals the mean, so the SD changes with the square root of the mean

As a result, an “ideal” observer would follow the

Theory and Practice In practice: at low background intensities, human observers behave as an ideal detector (follow the deVries-Rose Law)

Fig. 3.1

You always can determine the Weber fraction, even when Weber’s Law does not hold

Weber’s Law holds Weber’s Law does NOT hold (  L/ L rises as L decreases) Fig. 3.2

Both the deVries-Rose and Weber’s laws fail to account for thresholds at high light intensities The increment threshold data of a rod monochromat (circles) plotted along with the theoretical lower limit (deVries-Rose, dotted line) and the predictions of Weber’s Law (solid line). Luminance values are in cd/m 2. (Redrawn from Hess et al. (1990) Fig. 3.3

More practical issues: How changes in other stimulus dimensions affect the Weber fraction

Fig. 3.4 #1 Stimulus size: the Weber fraction is lower (smaller) for larger test stimuli

Log Background Intensity, L (cd/m 2 ) Log Weber Fraction,  L/L ' 4' Test Field Diameter More practical issues: Is a target visible under certain conditions? Is a spot with a particular luminance, relative to background, visible? It depends on its size. This is the target’s Weber fraction. It is NOT a threshold If the target is 121’, it is visible If 4’, it is not visible

Need to distinguish between the Weber fraction of a target vs. the threshold of a viewer. For a subject or patient viewing a target, if the subject’s Weber fraction is below a line, then the subject’s threshold is better (smaller). If the Weber fraction of a target is below the line, the target is NOT visible to someone whose threshold is on the line.

The smaller the threshold  L, the smaller is the value of the Weber fraction for a given background L, (only the numerator changes) and the more sensitive the visual system is to differences in light intensity.

Fig. 1.2

The “dinner plate” example: Plate with luminance of footlamberts. Background is 0.01 footlamberts  L is thus – 0.01 =  L/L = /0.01 = 0.02 (plate is 2% more intense) From Figure 3-4, can learn that this is not visible.

.

Log Background Intensity, L (cd/m 2 ) Log Weber Fraction,  L/L ' 4' Test Field Diameter More practical issues: Is a target visible under certain conditions? Is a spot with a particular luminance, relative to background, visible? It depends on its size. Plate’s Weber fraction Threshold Weber fraction for 121’ objects

#2 Short-duration flashes are harder to see (are less discriminable) than long-duration flashes That is, the threshold  L increases as flash duration becomes shorter. Continuing: How changes in other stimulus dimensions affect the Weber fraction

Sensitivity = 1/threshold

#3 Threshold  L varies with eccentricity from the fovea At low luminance levels, threshold is lowest (sensitivity is highest) about degrees from fovea and the fovea is “blind” At high luminance levels, threshold is lowest at the fovea Continuing: How changes in other stimulus dimensions affect the Weber fraction

Fig. 3.5 This is the basis for visual field tests

Sensory Magnitude Scales Revisited Using the “just noticeable difference” (jnd) to create a scale for sensory magnitude vs. stimulus magnitude L + threshold  L = L T L T is one jnd more intense than L. L T + threshold  L = L T2 L T2 is one jnd more intense than L T And so on…

Stimulus Luminance, L (cd/m 2 ) Sensory Magnitude L

Stimulus Luminance, L (cd/m 2 ) Sensory Magnitude LTLT L L + threshold  L = L T L T is one “just noticeable difference” (jnd) more intense than L.

Stimulus Luminance, L (cd/m 2 ) Sensory Magnitude L T + threshold  L = L T2 L T2 is one jnd more intense than L T and 2 jnd’s larger than L LTLT L L T2

Stimulus Luminance, L (cd/m 2 ) Sensory Magnitude When Weber’s Law holds, the threshold  Ls keep getting larger, so 1 jnd is a larger increase in stimulus luminance LTLT L L T2 L Tn L Tn+1

Stimulus Luminance, L (cd/m 2 ) Sensory Magnitude Fechner's Law: Log(L) Fechner’s Law

Comparing Fechner’s Law with Stevens’ Power Law Stevens’ Power Law resembles Fechner’s Law when the exponent is <1 Fig. 3.6