Download presentation
Presentation is loading. Please wait.
Published byOpal Harmon Modified over 7 years ago
1
Example trial sequences for visual perspective-taking task
Anxiety Impairs Spontaneous Perspective Calculation Austin J. Simpson & Andrew R. Todd University of Iowa Introduction Although reasoning about others’ mental states is central to social life, adults commonly make perspective-taking errors. These errors are heightened when people are experiencing particular emotions (e.g., anxiety; Todd et al., 2015) What cognitive mechanisms underlie anxiety-induced perspective-taking difficulties? Ascribing mental states may involve two distinct process (Leslie et al., 2005) Implicit calculation: What does a person see, know, or want? Explicit selection: Which calculated perspective is correct? These processes can be separated in visual perspective-taking (VPT) tasks (Samson et al., 2010) Two intrusion effects commonly emerge in VPT tasks: Egocentric intrusion: More difficulty reporting other perspective when it doesn’t align with one’s own Altercentric intrusion: More difficulty reporting own perspective when it doesn’t align with another’s How does feeling anxious affect these processes? Are these effects exclusive to social contexts? Hypotheses Perspective-calculation hypothesis: Anxiety increases egocentric intrusion and decreases altercentric intrusion Past research shows increased egocentrism with anxiety (Todd et al., 2015) Perspective-selection hypothesis: Deficits in executive function from anxiety (e.g., Eysenck et al., 2007) lead to increases in both egocentric and altercentric intrusion Method Experiment 1 147 undergraduates (82 women, 63 men, 3 unreported) Emotion recall task: Anxiety (Cataldo & Cohen, 2015) or neutral-emotion VPT task (Samson et al., 2010) Four equivalent blocks of 52 experimental trials Trial types: Other versus self trials; Consistent versus inconsistent trials Fixation cross (500 ms) Perspective cue (“YOU” or “HE”) (750 ms) Number of dots cue (0-3) (750 ms) Response (Does number of dots cue match or mismatch number of dots visible from cued perspective?) Experiment 2a 106 undergraduates (76 women, 30 men) Emotion recall task: Anxiety or anger Experiment 2b Non-social variant of VPT task (Surtees & Apperly, 2012) Human avatar replaced with dual-colored stick Discussion Results support the perspective-calculation account, whereby anxiety increases egocentric intrusion and decreases altercentric intrusion Conducted internal meta-analysis (see Borenstein et al., 2009) Anxiety marginally increased egocentric intrusion, g=0.22, 90% CI: [0.02, 0.43], Z=1.77, p=.077 Anxiety significantly decreased altercentric intrusion, g=-0.26, 95% CI: [-0.50, -0.01], Z=2.06, p=.040 Anxiety’s disruptive effects on cognitive performance are especially pronounced for social aspects of cognition Future Directions Do similar results emerge on indirect measures that assess the implicit tracking of higher-order mental states (e.g., beliefs)? Do integral (vs. incidental, as shown here) forms of anxiety similarly affect perspective-taking processes? Conclusion These findings enhance our understanding of how anxiety shapes social aspects of cognition The current work suggests that feeling anxious can undermine spontaneous tendencies to track others’ perspectives References Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester, West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons. Cataldo, A. M.., & Cohen, A. L. (2015). The effect of emotional state on visual detection: A signal detection analysis. Emotion, 15, 846–853. Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., & Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion, 7, 336–353. Qureshi, A. W., Apperly, I. A., & Samson, D. (2010). Executive function is necessary for perspective selection, not level-1 visual perspective calculation: Evidence from a dual-task study of adults. Cognition, 117, 230–236. Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Braithwaite, J. J., Andrews, B. J., & Bodley Scott, S. E. (2010). Seeing it their way: Evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1255–1266. Surtees, A. D. R., & Apperly, I. A. (2012). Egocentrism and automatic perspective taking in children and adults. Child Development, 83, 452– 460. Todd, A. R., Forstmann, M., Burgmer, P., Brooks, A. W., & Galinsky, A. D. (2015). Anxious and egocentric: How specific emotions influence perspective taking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 374–391. Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1983). Stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Example trial sequences for visual perspective-taking task Intrusion effects by condition (Experiments 2a & 2b) Results Exclusions: Excluded mismatch trials and response times (RTs) <200 ms and >2000 ms Processing cost metric (Qureshi et al., 2010; Townsend & Ashby, 1983): Integrates speed and accuracy into single metric [Mean correct RTs/Proportion of correct responses] Experiment 1 Data support the perspective-calculation hypothesis—Emotion x Perspective x Consistency: F(1, 145)=4.34, p=.039, ηp2=.03 Underlying pattern (egocentric intrusion > altercentric intrusion) stronger for anxious, F(1, 75)=41.45, p<.001, ηp2=.36, than neutral participants, F(1, 70)=11.59, p=.001, ηp2 =.14 Experiments 2a and 2b Data again support the perspective-calculation hypothesis—Emotion x Perspective x Consistency: F(1, 104)=6.05, p=.016, ηp2=.06 stronger for anxious, F(1, 53)=44.50, p<.001, ηp2=.46, than angry participants, F(1, 51)=6.72, p=.012, ηp2 =.12 Anxiety’s effects are limited to situations involving social stimuli (i.e., not observed with non-social avatar in Experiment 2b), all relevant Fs < 1 Intrusion effects by condition (Experiment 1)
Similar presentations
© 2025 SlidePlayer.com. Inc.
All rights reserved.