Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

PAIN & INTEROCEPTIVE BODY–AWARENESS

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "PAIN & INTEROCEPTIVE BODY–AWARENESS"— Presentation transcript:

1 PAIN & INTEROCEPTIVE BODY–AWARENESS
Understanding of pain-related questions differs between primary care pain patients and mind-body trained individuals Mehling W, Bartmess E, Acree M, Price C, Daubenmier J, Gopisetty V, Stewart A RESULTS BACKGROUND Table 2: Pain/Discomfort Questionnaire Items: Mean Values ± SD* Interoceptive Body-Awareness is a construct with multiple dimensions: ● noticing body sensations incl. pain, discomfort ● behavioral reaction incl. ignoring, distraction ● emotional reaction incl. worry, upset ● attention regulation incl. mindfulness ● awareness of emotion-related sensations, ● behavioral and emotional self-regulation, ● and trust in meaningfulness of sensations. Pain and Interoceptive Body-Awareness ● Ignoring pain (incl. distraction from pain) may be beneficial or maladaptive. ● Mind-body approaches, e.g. Yoga, Tai Chi, meditation, Feldenkrais, Alexander teach mindful body-awareness and not ignoring body sensations; preliminary studies of these approaches suggest benefits for pain. Question: Do individuals trained in mind- body approaches respond differently to pain-related questions than pain patients in primary care? Ignoring Pain or Discomfort Pain Mind-Body A I do not notice physical tension or discomfort until they become more severe 2.8 ±1.4 1.6 ±1.1 B I distract myself from sensations of discomfort 2.8 ±1.5 1.8 ±1.1 C When feeling pain or discomfort, I try to power through it 3.6 ±1.3 1.9 ±1.2 Worry with Pain or Discomfort D When I feel physical pain, I become upset 2.4 ±1.5 2.0 ±1.1 E I start to worry that something is wrong if I feel any discomfort 2.1 ±1.5 1.5 ±1.1 F I can notice an unpleasant body sensation without worrying about it (reversed) 1.8 ±1.4 1.6 ±1.0 Body and Pain Awareness with Mindfulness (selected items from subscale for attention regulation) G I can be aware of pain or tension without actively trying to change it 3.4 ±1.3 3.6 ±0.9 H I can maintain awareness of my whole body even when a part of me is in pain or discomfort 3.0 ±1.3 3.5 ±0.9 Awareness of Pain or Discomfort (selected items from subscale for noticing body sensations) I When I am tense I notice where the tension is located in my body 3.4 ±1.6 4.0 ±0.7 J I notice when I am uncomfortable in my body 4.0 ±1.3 4.3 ±0.7 Subscale Scores Pain** Mind-Body Table 1: Demographics Pain Mind-Body Gender (% female)* 53 79 Average age* 54 ±12 48 ±12 Ethnicity (%)* Asian-American 11 3 African-American 8 2 Latino 6 4 White 68 83 Other 7 Education (%)* High school 9 <1 Some college 26 College degree 37 Graduate school 28 N 436 307 *Group differences significant at p< .001 Scale Range: 0 (never) to 5 (always) for how often each statement applies to you in daily life. * NOTE: All group differences in item and subscale means are significant at p<.001, except J p=0.12, controlled for age, gender, ethnicity, education **Bars: subscale means, 95% CI. ● Pain-Related Item and Sub-Scale Means (Table 2): - Pain patients report more frequently ignoring pain or discomfort compared with mind-body trained individuals. - Pain patients report being more often worried when feeling pain or discomfort than did mind-body trained individuals. - Pain patients scored “worse” on all pain-related body-awareness items and subscales compared with mind-body trained individuals. - Pain patients with current chronic pain did not differ from patients with past acute pain (2 years earlier) except for items E and H, where they scored “better” than patients with past acute pain (data not shown). ● Exploratory Factor Analysis: In an analysis with all MAIA subscales, items related to ignoring pain or discomfort factor-loaded together with items related to worry in mind-body trained individuals, but loaded separately in pain patients. ● MAIA Subscale Correlations were similar in both groups except “ignoring” and “worry”: in pain patients these did not correlate with each other (r = -.11) or with any other MAIA sub-scale (r <.20). However, in mind-body trained individuals, lower “ignoring” and “worry” scores correlated with higher scores on 3 other MAIA subscales for attention regulation, emotion & behavior self-regulation, and trust in meaningfulness of body sensations (r -.20 to -.35, p<.001). METHODS We compared: ● pain-related item and sub-scale means ● factor structure (EFAs) ● sub-scale correlations using a new Self-Report measure: Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA) Group1 (“Pain”): 436 pain patients seen in primary care 2 years after acute low back pain (19% of this group still in chronic pain), representative of the usual population seen in primary care Group2 (“Mind-Body”): 307 healthy indivi- duals trained in mind-body therapies CONCLUSIONS ● Data suggest that primary care patients with current or past pain endorse a common ignoring coping style in response to pain and discomfort, whereas mind-body trained individuals may have learned a new coping style. ● Data suggest that mind-body trained individuals use an alternate coping style of less ignoring and less worry about pain or discomfort than common pain patients; it goes along with greater self-reported skills in self-regulation of behavior and emotions. ● SUMMARY: Cross-sectional data suggest that mind-body trained individuals, different from primary care patients with past or current pain, may have learned a new style of coping with pain / discomfort. Whether this is true deserves further (longitudinal) study. Research funding: NIH R21-AT004467 European Federation of IASP Chapters, Hamburg 9/2011


Download ppt "PAIN & INTEROCEPTIVE BODY–AWARENESS"

Similar presentations


Ads by Google