Download presentation
Presentation is loading. Please wait.
Published byKarin Kelly Modified over 6 years ago
1
Group Analyses in fMRI Jody Culham Brain and Mind Institute
Department of Psychology Western University Group Analyses in fMRI Last Update: November 21, 2016 Last Course: Psychology 9223, F2016, Western University
2
Group Analyses 1. Get all the subjects’ brains into a common space
Talairach space MNI space cortex-based alignment 2A. Do group statistics Random Effects GLM and/or 2B. Use a Region of Interest Approach
3
Combining Group Data
4
Brains are Heterogeneous
Slide from Duke course
5
Talairach Coordinate System
Talairach & Tournoux, 1988 made an atlas based on one brain any brain can be squished or stretched to fit hers and locations can be described using a 3D coordinate system (x, y, z) … from an alcoholic old lady Note: That’s TalAIRach, not TAILarach!
6
Rotate brain into ACPC plane
Find anterior commisure (AC) Corpus Callosum Fornix Find posterior commisure (PC) ACPC line = horizontal axis Pineal Body “bent asparagus” Note: official Tal says to use top of AC and bottom of PC but I suspect few people actually do this Source: Duvernoy, 1999
7
AC = Origin x = 0 + y = 0 z z = 0 - L R + y - - + x
8
Left is what?!!! L R R L Neurologic (i.e. sensible) convention
left is left, right is right L R x = 0 - + Note: Make sure you know what your magnet and software are doing before publishing left/right info! Radiologic (i.e. stupid) convention left is right, right is left R L
9
Tip Put a vitamin E capsule on the one side of the subject’s head or coil.
10
Deform brain into Talairach space
Mark 8 points in the brain: anterior commisure posterior commisure front back top bottom (of temporal lobe) left right Squish or stretch brain to fit in “shoebox” of Tal system ACPC=0 y>0 y<0 z y AC=0 y>0 y<0 x (left < 0; right > 0) Extract 3 coordinates
11
Talairach Tables Source: Culham et al., 2003, Exp. Brain Res. Talairach coordinates can be useful for other people to check whether their activation foci are similar to yours Often it’s easiest to just put coordinates in a table to avoid cluttering text
12
Do We need a “Tarailach Atras”?
Variability between Japanese and European brains, both male (red > yellow > green > blue) Variability between male and female brains, both European Source: Zilles et al., 2001, NeuroImage
13
Talairach Pros and Cons
Advantages widespread system allows averaging of fMRI data between subjects allows researchers to compare activation foci relatively easy to use Disadvantages does better for central regions of cortex, but not great for most of cortex not appropriate for all brains (e.g., group variability, patients may not fit well) ignores left- vs. right-hemisphere asymmetries activation foci can vary considerably – other landmarks like sulci may be more reliable
14
MNI Space Researchers at the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) created a better template based on a morphed average of hundreds of brains many different versions
15
MNI Space Benefits MNI Space is based on many subjects not just one brain like Talairach space MNI transformations use nonlinear warping, which leads to better intersubject alignment Image Source
16
Converting Between MNI and Tal space
You many want to convert between the systems Only Tal provides Brodmann’s areas Need to convert for meta-analyses The MNI and Talairach coordinates are similar but not identical e.g., temporal lobes extend 10 mm lower in MNI brain Caveat: careful comparison requires a transformation -- converters can be found online Source:
17
Anatomical Localization Sulci and Gyri
gray matter (dendrites & synapses) white matter (axons) FUNDUS BANK GYRUS SULCUS CROWN pial surface gray/white border neuron SULCUS FISSURE GYRUS Source: Ludwig & Klingler, 1956, in Tamraz & Comair, 2000
18
Variability of Sulci Source: Szikla et al., 1977, in Tamraz & Comair, 2000
19
Effects of Sulcal Variability
Source: Frost & Goebel, 2012, NeuroImage
20
Variability of Functional Areas
Watson et al., 1995 - motion-selective area, MT+ (=V5) is quite variable in stereotaxic space however, the area is quite consistent in its location relative to sulci junction of inferior temporal sulcus and lateral occipital sulcus see also Dumoulin et al., 2000
21
Cerebral cortex is a crumpled map
22
Cortical Surfaces segment gray-white matter boundary
render cortical surface inflate cortical surface sulci = concave = dark gray gyri = convex = light gray
23
Cortical Inflation Movie
Movie: unfoldorig.mpeg Source: Marty Sereno’s web page
24
Cortical Flattening Source: Brain Voyager Getting Started Guide
2) make cuts along the medial surface (Note, one cut typically goes along the fundus of the calcarine sulcus though in this example the cut was placed below) 1) inflate the brain 3) unfold the medial surface so the cortical surface lies flat 4) correct for the distortions so that the true cortical distances are preseved Source: Brain Voyager Getting Started Guide
25
Spherical Averaging Future directions of fMRI: Use cortical surface mapping coordinates Inflate the brain into a sphere Use sulci and/or functional areas to match subject’s data to template Cite “latitude” & “longitude” of spherical coordinates Source: Fischl et al., 1999
26
Spherical Averaging Source: Fischl et al., 1999
Movie: brain2ellipse.mpeg Source: Marty Sereno’s web page Movie: morph-curv1.mpg Source: Marty Sereno’s web page Source: Fischl et al., 1999
27
Before and After CBA Source: Frost & Goebel, 2012, NeuroImage
28
Before and After CBA hand motor area (M1) hand somatosensory area (S1)
Source: Frost & Goebel, 2012, NeuroImage
29
Gains in Overlap Source: Frost & Goebel, 2012, NeuroImage
30
Voxelwise Group Analyses
Fixed vs. Random Effects
31
Example Three subjects Three conditions: Baseline, Faces, Objects
For simplicity, just consider one voxel in FFA
32
Stupid Way: Concatenated Fixed Effects (FFX)
Concatenate data, pretend you have one subject who did three runs
33
Stupid Way: Concatenated Fixed Effects (FFX)
Make one predictor for Faces and one for Objects (2 df) Scale predictors (by beta weights) Note why this is stupid Assumes all subject show same magnitude of activation Errors in this assumption residuals
34
Better Way: Separate Subjects FFX
Don’t concatenate Separate predictors for 3 subjects x 2 conditions 6 df
35
Problem: Separate Subjects FFX
We could do business-as-usual GLM and see if predictors account for significant variance considering noise Effectively, we are asking how confident we are that this effect is true (not due to chance) in these three subjects (and only these three subjects) BUT usually, we want to generalize to the population from which we sampled
36
Best Way: Random Effects (RFX)
37
Second-level analysis
Nothing too complicated… it’s effectively just a paired t-test Subject βFaces βObjects Difference βFaces - βObjects S1 0.552 0.105 0.447 S2 2.061 1.121 0.940 S3 1.019 0.247 0.772 Mean 0.719 SD 0.250 SEM [=SD/sqrt(N)] 0.145 tcrit(df=3) 4.3 95%CI lower (= mean – (t*SEM)) 0.097 95%CI lower (= mean + (t*SEM)) 1.34 Estimated Distribution of Differences does not include zero significant (p<.05) 0.719 0.097 1.34 95% CI
38
Repeat for the other 60,000 voxels…
Huettel, Song & McCarthy, 2008 First-level analysis Second-level analysis
39
If you really want to be correct…
We often refer to this type of analysis as random effects (RFX) Since subjects is a random effect but other aspects (e.g., stimulus categories) are fixed effects, technically the proper term is Mixed Effects Analysis Other common jargon = Second-level Analysis
40
Take-home Message RFX enables us to generalize to the population from which we sampled subjects Degrees of freedom comes from number of subjects, not number of time points No need to worry about correction for serial correlations for most fMRI studies, this means underpaid graduate students in need of a few bucks!
41
Examples from a real data set
42
Concatenated FFX Example 17 Subjects x 2 runs with Faces & Houses
df = 17 Ss x 2 runs/S x 264 vols/run - 1 one predictor per condition
43
FFX Separate Subjects …
If you’re looking at pilot data from a few Ss, with RFX it will be hard to see any effects. You can do FFX with separate subjects. It has the same problems as concatenated FFX but at least enables you to examine the consistency between Ss
44
Now that our df no longer depends on # volumes,
RFX S1 S2 S3 … S17 This contrast is just like doing a paired t-test between Faces and Objects with 17 Ss df = 17 Ss - 1 Now that our df no longer depends on # volumes, we don’t have to worry about correction for serial correlations with RFX
45
The Problem of Intersubject Variability
The hand area lies in the “hand knob”, a section of the central sulcus that looks like an upside-down omega Primary Motor Cortex (M1) lies along the anterior bank while Primary Somatosensory Cortex (S1) lies along the posterior bank Ω Ω 2+ cm Although these areas are reliably localized with respect to sulcal landmarks within individuals, the sulcus has high anatomical variability between individuals
46
The Problem of Intersubject Variability
The hand area lies in the “hand knob”, a section of the central sulcus that looks like an upside-down omega Primary Motor Cortex (M1) lies along the anterior bank while Primary Somatosensory Cortex (S1) lies along the posterior bank Although these areas are reliably localized with respect to sulcal landmarks within individuals, the sulcus has high anatomical variability between individuals Due to this variability, activation of the same area (e.g., the motor hand area during finger tapping) can appear scattered in stereotaxic (Talairach or MNI) space. Given that random effects (RFX) analyses require somewhat consistent effects across participants, this can handicap statistical power This also means that it can be tricky to isolate effects in one area (e.g., M1) without contamination from adjacent areas (e.g., S1) Ω Ω S1 β = 3 S2 β = 0 S3 β = 0 RFX non-sig S1 S2 S3 3 Ss superimposed
47
Three Solutions to Intersubject Variability
RFX non-sig S1 β = 2.5 S2 β = 2.5 S3 β = 1 RFX sig Solution 1: Spatial Smoothing increase likelihood of consistent effects across Ss doesn’t solve problem that voxels likely contain combination of different areas (M1 and S1 in this case) Solution 2: Cortex-based Alignment solution works very well and makes it less likely to blur across adjacent functional areas generation of surfaces can be time-consuming (though automation is getting better) cortex-centric S1 β = 3 S2 β = 3 S3 β = 3 RFX highly sig S1 S2 S3 Solution 3: Individual ROIs identify ROI anatomically in each S (e.g., based on anterior bank of hand knob) or functionally in each S (based on functional localizer) anatomical ROIs can require manual labour functional ROIs can be somewhat subjective doesn’t work for unexpected activation sites S1 β = 3 S2 β = 3 S3 β = 3 RFX highly sig Original Data
48
Random Effects Analysis
Brain Voyager recommends you don’t even toy with random effects unless you’ve got 10 or more subjects (and 50+ is best) Random effects analyses can really squash your data, especially if you don’t have many subjects. Though standards were lower in the early days of fMRI, today it’s virtually impossible to publish any group voxelwise data without RFX analysis
49
Strategies for Exploration vs. Publication
Deductive approach Have a specific hypothesis/contrast planned Run all your subjects Run the stats as planned Publish Inductive approach Run a few subjects to see if you’re on the right track Spend a lot of time exploring the pilot data for interesting patterns “Find the story” in the data You may even change the experiment, run additional subjects, or run a follow-up experiment to chase the story While you need to use rigorous corrections for publication, do not be overly conservative when exploring pilot data or you might miss interesting trends Random effects analyses can be quite conservative so you may want to do exploratory analyses with fixed effects (and then run more subjects if needed so you can publish random effects)
50
How can we identify activation foci?
Talairach coordinates Example: The FFA is at x = 40, y = -55, z = -10 Anatomical localization Example: The FFA is in the right fusiform gyrus at the level of the occipitotemporal junction Functional localization Example: The FFA includes all voxels around the fusiform gyrus that are activated by the comparison between faces and objects Kanwisher, McDermott & Chun, 1997, J Neurosci
51
Talairach Daemon
52
Neurosynth Neurosynth (Dec. 2014)
53
Automated Coordinate Extraction
Yarkoni et al., 2001
54
Example: Working Memory
META- ANALYSIS from previous studies FORWARD INFERENCE What is the probability of activation here a study involving working memory? REVERSE INFERENCE If there is activation here, what is the probability the study involved working memory? Yarkoni et al., 2001
55
Automated Coordinate Extraction
Yarkoni et al., 2001
56
Definition of an “Area”
Neuroimager’s definition of an area: Some blob vaguely in the vicinity (+/- ~3 cm) of where I think it ought to be that lights up for something I think it ought to light up for Neuroanatomist’s definition of an area: A circumscribed region of the cerebral cortex in which neurons together serve a specific function, receive connections from the same regions, have a common structural arrangement, and in some cases show a topographic arrangement may also be called a cortical field
57
Cortical Fields: Multiple Criteria
Function an area has a unique pattern of responses to different stimuli Architecture different brain areas show differences between cortical properties (e.g., thickness of different layers, sensitivity to various dyes) Connectivity Different areas have different patterns of connections with other areas Topography many sensory areas show topography (retinotopy, somatotopy, tonotopy) boundaries between topographic maps can indicate boundaries between areas (e.g., separate maps of visual space in visual areas V1 and V2)
58
Can We Use Multiple Criteria in Human Imaging?
Function this is often the only criterion in fMRI Architecture there are now probabilistic maps of human brain areas available (Zilles lab) Connectivity DTI and functional connectivity now give us options here Topography topography is useful in imaging, especially for early and mid-level sensory areas
59
Brodmann Area 17 Meets 21st Century
Anatomical MRI Functional MRI Layer 4 Logothetis fMRI data: image from Goense, Zappe & Logothetis, 2007, MRI Layer 4 fMRI activation (0.3 x 0.3 x 2 mm spin echo)
60
Brodmann’s Areas Brodmann (1905):
Based on cytoarchitectonics: study of differences in cortical layers between areas Most common delineation of cortical areas More recent schemes subdivide Brodmann’s areas into many smaller regions Monkey and human Brodmann’s areas not necessarily homologous
61
Brodmann Area 17
62
Retinotopic Maps EXPANDING RINGS ROTATING WEDGES
63
DTI in V1 Saentz & Fine, 2010, NeuroImage
64
Maps, Maps, Maps … even in parietal lobe … … even in frontal lobe …
Hagler & Sereno, 2006, NeuroImage Wandell et al., 2007, Neuron
65
Other Sensory “-topies”
Touch: Somatotopy Servos et al., 1998 red = wrist; orange = shoulder Audition: Tonotopy Sylvian fissure temporal lobe cochlea Movie: tonotopy.mpeg Source: Marty Sereno’s web page
66
Learning Brain Anatomy
Duvernoy, 1999, The Human Brain: Surface, Blood Supply, and Three-Dimensional Sectional Anatomy beautiful pictures good schematic diagrams clear anatomy slices of real brain Springer, US$439 DISCONTINUED Ono, 1990, Atlas of the Cerebral Sulci great for showing intersubject variability gives probabilities of configurations and stats on sulci Theime, US$199 Damasio,2005, Human Brain Anatomy in Computerized Images, 2nd edition good for showing sulci across wide range of slice planes 2nd edition much better than 1st edition Oxford University Press, US$100 Tamraz & Comair, 2000, Atlas of Regional Anatomy of the Brain Using MRI with Functional Correlations good overview Springer, US$203 Talairach & Tournoux, Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain just because it’s the standard doesn’t mean it’s good Theime, US$240
67
Brain Tutor Mac/PC: free iOS App: $1.99
68
Proposal Guidelines Jody Culham Brain and Mind Institute
Department of Psychology Western University Proposal Guidelines Last Update: November 21, 2016 Last Course: Psychology 9223, F2016, Western University
69
Research Proposal Due December 12, 2016
Goals give students an opportunity to demonstrate what they’ve learned and apply ideas to their research area give students practice in writing grants/papers 16 double-spaced pages + figures must be original not thesis not something your advisor totally worked out for you can get some suggestions from advisor but core of proposal should be your work
70
Research Proposal partially like a grant partially like a paper
proposal for experiment make case for why experiment should be done “hasn’t been done before” is not good enough clear question, hypotheses conclusion: so what? immunization against potential criticism partially like a paper just one experiment, not 5 years of experiments in-depth methods be clear about design (e.g., protocol) and analyses be clear about contrasts Appendix with budget and time line don’t worry about formatting – spend your time on content not formatting
71
Range of Approaches Standard univariate fMRI with hypothesis-driven GLM Block or Event-related Advanced designs e.g., MVPA Data-driven fMRI e.g., ICA on resting state data Approaches we’ve touched on in class e.g., intersubject correlations Anatomical approaches DTI For the more computationally inclined better ways to analyze data if you must use equations, explain them intuitively in text (and consider putting them in an appendix) Be aware that we won’t discuss these in too much detail in class; therefore you would need to have some prior exposure or to do some extra reading
72
Two questions to consider whenever you write a paper or give a talk
Who is my audience? a math-phobic professor who will be checking whether you understood the core ideas of fMRI What is my goal? show professor that you can find a way to use neuroimaging in your research show professor that you understand jargon and concepts Bonus be clever and creative write clearly and concisely solidify your understanding of neuroimaging approaches think more deeply about how to apply neuroimaging
73
Sections Introduction
Give enough information to put the research in context and lead the reader to the conclusion that the experiment you’re proposing is a reasonable next step You don’t need to cite every paper in the history of neuroimaging Do enough of a lit search to be fairly certain proposal hasn’t been done Replication attempts discouraged (but may be considered with sufficient justification) Methods Include enough detail to demonstrate that you understand jargon and key concepts Be clear about specific contrasts Results How could it turn out? May want to include graph of hypotheses Conclusions Don’t just summarize; conclude! So what?! What would it mean if the results turned out one way or another? Are there any caveats that should be acknowledged? What is the broader significance of the research? References whatever format you like (e.g., APA) don’t go overboard Figures (optional) Appendix How much will it cost? How long will it take?
74
Key Methods # participants (pilot + discarded + final)
Design (block, event-related) Timing of TR, trials, blocks, jittering, runs, session Stimulus/task conditions + rationale Preprocessing steps include criterion for motion correction Approach (localizer?; ROI and/or voxelwise; individual and/or group; for event-related convolution or deconvolution) Contrasts don’t just say “activation for faces” or “activation for faces vs houses, bodies & scrambled” define specific contrast: e.g., “a conjunction of faces vs. each control category [(F>H)AND(F>B)AND(F>S)] corrections (esp. multiple comparisons)
75
Example of Hypothesis Figure
Similar presentations
© 2025 SlidePlayer.com. Inc.
All rights reserved.