Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Chapter 2: Introduction to the Physiology of Perception

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Chapter 2: Introduction to the Physiology of Perception"— Presentation transcript:

1 Chapter 2: Introduction to the Physiology of Perception
Chapter 2: Introduction to the Physiology of Perception

2 Overview of Questions How are physiological processes involved in perception? How can electrical signals in the nervous system represent objects in the environment?

3 The Brain: History of the Physiological Approach
Aristotle ( B.C.) - heart was seat of mind and soul Galen ( A.D.) - “spirits” flowed through ventricles of brain Descartes (1630s) - pineal gland was seat of soul Willis (1664) - beginning of modern view that the brain is responsible for mental functions

4 The Brain: History - continued
Mueller (1842) - doctrine of specific nerve energies Golgi (1873) - developed method of staining specific neurons Adrian (1920s) - recordings from single neurons 1950s - modern era of brain research began

5 Figure 2.1 Some notable ideas and events regarding the physiological workings of the mind.

6 Basic Brain Structure The brain has modular organization The sensory modalities have primary receiving areas Vision - occipital lobe Audition - temporal lobe Tactile senses - parietal lobe Frontal lobe coordinates information received from two or more senses

7 Figure 2.3 The human brain, showing the locations of the primary receiving areas for the senses in the temporal, occipital, and parietal lobes, and the frontal lobe, which is involved with integrating sensory functions.

8 Neurons: Transduction &Transmission
Key components of neurons: Cell body Dendrites Axon or nerve fiber Receptors - specialized neurons that respond to specific kinds of energy

9 Figure 2.4 The neuron on the right consists of a cell body, dendrites, and an axon, or nerve fiber. The neuron on the left that receives stimuli from the environment has a receptor in place of the cell body.

10 Figure 2.5 Receptors for (a) vision, (b) hearing, (c) touch, (d) smell, and (e) taste. Each of these receptors is specialized to transduce a specific type of environmental energy into electricity. Arrows indicate the place on the receptor neuron where the stimulus acts to begin the process of transduction.

11 Recording Neural Signals
Microelectrodes are used to record from single neurons. Recording electrode is inside the nerve fiber. Reference electrode is outside the fiber. Difference in charge between them is -70 mV This negative charge of the neuron relative to its surroundings is the resting potential.

12 Recording Neural Signals - continued
Electrical signals or action potentials occur when: permeability of the membrane changes Na+ flows into the fiber making the neuron more positive K+ flows out of the fiber making the neuron more negative This process travels down the axon in a propagated response

13 Figure 2.7 (a) When a nerve fiber is at rest, there is a difference in charge of -70 mV between the inside and the outside of the fiber. This difference is measured by the meter on the left; the difference in charge measured by the meter is displayed on the right. (b) As the nerve impulse, indicated by the gray band, passes the electrode, the inside of the fiber near the electrode becomes more positive. This positivity is the rising phase of the action potential. (c) As the nerve impulse moves past the electrode, the charge inside the fiber becomes more negative. This is the falling phase of the action potential. (d) Eventually the neuron returns to its resting state.

14 Basics of Neural Signals
Neurons are surrounded by a solution containing ions. Ions carry an electrical charge. Sodium ions (Na+) - positive charge Chlorine ions (Cl-) - negative charge Potassium ions (K+) - positive charge Electrical signals are generated when such ions cross the membranes of neurons. Membranes have selective permeability.

15 Figure 2.8 A nerve fiber, showing the high concentration of sodium outside the fiber and potassium inside the fiber. Other ions, such as negatively charged chlorine, are not shown.

16 Properties of Action Potentials
show propagated response. remain the same size regardless of stimulus intensity. increase in rate to increase in stimulus intensity. have a refractory period of 1 ms - upper firing rate is 500 to 800 impulses per second. show spontaneous activity that occurs without stimulation.

17 Figure 2.10 Response of a nerve fiber to (a) soft, (b) medium, and (c) strong stimulation. Increasing the stimulus strength increases both the rate and the regularity of nerve firing in this fiber.

18 Synaptic Transmission of Neural Impulses
Neurotransmitters are: released by the presynaptic neuron from vesicles. received by the postsynaptic neuron on receptor sites. matched like a key to a lock into specific receptor sites. used as triggers for voltage change in the postsynaptic neuron.

19 Figure 2. 11 Synaptic transmission from one neuron to another
Figure 2.11 Synaptic transmission from one neuron to another. (a) A signal traveling down the axon of a neuron reaches the synapse at the end of the axon. (b) The nerve impulse causes the release of neurotransmitter molecules from the synaptic vesicles of the sending neuron. (c) The neurotransmitters fit into receptor sites and cause a voltage change in the receiving neuron.

20 VIDEO: Synaptic Transmission

21 Types of Neurotransmitters
Excitatory transmitters - cause depolarization Neuron becomes more positive Increases the likelihood of an action potential Inhibitory transmitters - cause hyperpolarization Neuron becomes more negative Decreases the likelihood of an action potential

22 Figure 2.12 (a) Excitatory transmitters cause depolarization, an increased positive charge inside the neuron. (b) Inhibitory transmitters cause hyperpolization, an increased negative charge inside the axon. The charge inside the axon must reach the dashed line to trigger an action potential.

23 Figure 2.13 Effect of excitatory (E) and inhibitory (I) input on the firing rate of a neuron. The amount of excitatory and inhibitory input to the neuron is indicated by the size of the arrows at the synapse. The responses recorded by the electrode are indicated by the records on the right. The firing that occurs before the stimulus is presented is spontaneous activity. In (a) the neuron receives only excitatory transmitter, which causes the neuron to fire. In (b) to (e) the amount of excitatory transmitter decreases while the amount of inhibitory transmitter increases. As inhibition becomes stronger relative to excitation, firing rate decreases, until eventually the neuron stops firing.

24 Neural Circuits Groups of neurons connected by excitatory and inhibitory synapses A simple circuit has no convergence and only excitatory inputs. Input into each receptor has no effect on the output of neighboring circuits. Each circuit can only indicate single spot of stimulation.

25 Figure 2. 14 Left: A circuit with no convergence
Figure Left: A circuit with no convergence. Right: Response of neuron B as we increase the number of receptors stimulated.

26 Neural Circuits - continued
Convergent circuit with only excitatory connections Input from each receptor summates into the next neuron in the circuit. Output from convergent system varies based on input. Output of circuit can indicate single input and increases output as length of stimulus increases.

27 Figure 2. 15 Circuit with convergence added
Figure 2.15 Circuit with convergence added. Neuron B now receives inputs form all of the receptors, so increasing the size of the stimulus increases the size of neuron B’s response.

28 Neural Circuits - continued
Convergent circuit with excitatory and inhibitory connections Inputs from receptors summate to determine output of circuit. Summation of inputs result in: weak response for single inputs and long stimuli. maximum firing rate for medium length stimulus.

29 Figure 2. 16 Circuit with convergence and inhibition
Figure 2.16 Circuit with convergence and inhibition. Because stimulation of the receptors on the side (1, 2, 6, and 7) sends inhibition to neuron B, neuron B responds best when just the center (3 - 5) are stimulated.

30 Receptive fields are determined by monitoring single cell responses.
Area of receptors that affects firing rate of a given neuron in the circuit Receptive fields are determined by monitoring single cell responses. Research example for vision Stimulus is presented to retina and response of cell is measured by an electrode. Important to emphasize that the receptive field is on the retina. Students tend to forget this as you work your way through the explanation of more specifically tuned neurons further into the system.

31 Figure 2.17 Recording electrical signals from a fiber in the optic nerve of an anesthetized cat. Each point on the screen corresponds to a point on the cat’s retina.

32 Center-Surround Receptive Fields
Excitatory and inhibitory effects are found in receptive fields. Center and surround areas of receptive fields result in: Excitatory-center-inhibitory surround Inhibitory-center-excitatory surround

33 Figure (a) Response of a ganglion cell in the cat’s retina to stimulation: outside the cell’s receptive field (area A on the screen); inside the excitatory area of the cell’s receptive field (area B); and inside the inhibitory area of the cell’s receptive field (area C). (b) The receptive field is shown without the screen.

34 Center-Surround Antagonism
Output of center-surround receptive fields changes depending on area stimulated: Highest response when only the excitatory area is stimulated Lowest response when only the inhibitory area is stimulated Intermediate responses when both areas are stimulated

35 Figure Response of an excitatory-center-inhibitory-surround receptive field as stimulus size is increased. Shading indicates the area stimulated with light. The response to the stimulus is indicated below each receptive field. The largest response occurs when the entire excitatory area is illuminated, as in (b). Increasing stimulus size further causes a decrease in firing due to center-surround antagonism. (Adapted from Hubel and Wiesel, 1961.)

36 Sensory Code: Representation of Environment
Sensory code - representation of perceived objects through neural firing Specificity coding - specific neurons responding to specific stimuli Leads to the “grandmother cell” hypothesis Recent research shows cells in the hippocampus that respond to concepts such as Halle Berry.

37 Figure 2. 21 How faces could be coded by specificity coding
Figure 2.21 How faces could be coded by specificity coding. Each face causes one specialized neuron to respond.

38 Figure 2.22 (a) Location of the hippocampus and some of the other structures that were studied by Quiroga and coworkers (2005). (b) Some of the stimuli that caused a neuron in the hippocampus to fire.

39 Sensory Code: Representation of Environment - continued
Problems with specificity coding: Too many different stimuli to assign specific neurons Most neurons respond to a number of different stimuli. Distributed coding - pattern of firing across many neurons codes specific objects Large number of stimuli can be coded by a few neurons.

40 Figure 2. 23 How faces could be coded by distributed coding
Figure 2.23 How faces could be coded by distributed coding. Each face causes all the neurons to fire, but the pattern of firing is different for each face. One advantage of this method of coding is that many faces could be represented by the firing of the three neurons.

41 Sensory Code: Representation of Environment - continued
How many neurons are needed for an object in distributed coding? Sparse coding - only a relatively small number of neurons are necessary This theory can be viewed as a midpoint between specificity and distributed coding.

42 Mind-body Problem How do physiological processes become transformed into perceptual experience? Easy problem of consciousness Neural correlate of consciousness (NCC) - how physiological responses correlate with experience Hard problem of consciousness How do physiological responses cause experience?

43 Figure 2.24 (a) Solving the “easy” problem of consciousness involves looking for connections between physiological responding and experiences such as perceiving “red” or “John’s face.” This is also called the search for the neural correlate of consciousness. (b) Solving the “hard” problem of consciousness involves determining how physiological processes such as ions flowing across the nerve membrane cause us to have experiences.


Download ppt "Chapter 2: Introduction to the Physiology of Perception"

Similar presentations


Ads by Google