Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Data Mining – Credibility: Evaluating What’s Been Learned

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Data Mining – Credibility: Evaluating What’s Been Learned"— Presentation transcript:

1 Data Mining – Credibility: Evaluating What’s Been Learned
Chapter 5 Some of this was already covered

2 Evaluation Performance on training data is not representative – cheating – has seen all test instances during training If test involves testing on training data KNN with K=1 is the best technique !!!!!! Simplest fair evaluation = large training data AND large test data We have been using 10-fold cross-validation extensively – not just fair, also more likely to be accurate – less chance of unlucky or lucky results Better – repeated cross validation (as in experimenter environment in WEKA) – this allows statistical tests

3 Validation Data Some learning schemes involve testing what has been learned on other data – AS PART OF THEIR TRAINING !! Frequently, this process is used to “tune” parameters that can be adjusted in the method to obtain the best performance (e.g. threshold for accepting rule in Prism) The test during learning cannot be done on training data or test data Using training data would mean that the learning is being checked against data it has already seen Using test data would mean that the test data would have already been seen during (part of) learning Separate (3rd) data set should be used – “Validation”

4 Confidence Intervals If experiment shows 75% correct, we might be interested in what the correctness rate can actually be expected to be (the experiment is a result of sampling) We can develop a confidence interval around the result Skip Math

5 Cross-Validation Foundation is a simple idea – “holdout” – holds out a certain amount for testing and uses rest for training Separation should NOT be “convenience”, Should at least be random Better – “stratified” random – division preserves relative proportion of classes in both training and test data Enhanced : repeated holdout Enables using more data in training, while still getting a good test 10-fold cross validation has become standard This is improved if the folds are chosen in a “stratified” random way … simple holdout – save 1/3 for testing, 2/3 for training

6 Repeated Cross Validation
Folds in cross validation are not independent sample Contents of one fold are influenced by contents of other folds No instances in common So statistical tests (e.g. T Test) are not appropriate If you do repeated cross validation, the different cross validations are independent samples – folds drawn for one are different from others Will get some variation in results Any good / bad luck in forming of folds is averaged out Statistical tests are appropriate Becoming common to run fold cross validations Supported by experimenter environment in WEKA

7 For Small Datasets Leave One Out Bootstrapping To be discussed in turn

8 Leave One Out Train on all but one instance, test on that one (pct correct always equals 100% or 0%) Repeat until have tested on all instances, average results Really equivalent to N-fold cross validation where N = number of instances available Plusses: Always trains on maximum possible training data (without cheating) Efficient to run – no repeated (since fold contents not randomized) No stratification, no random sampling necessary Minuses Guarantees a non-stratified sample – the correct class will always be at least a little bit under-represented in the training data Statistical tests are not appropriate

9 Bootstrapping Sampling done with replacement to form a training dataset Particular approach – bootstrap Dataset of n instances is sampled n times Some instances will be included multiple times Those not picked will be used as test data On large enough dataset, .632 of the data instances will end up in the training dataset, rest will be in test This is a bit of a pessimistic estimate of performance, since only using 63% of data for training (vs 90% in 10-fold cross validation) May try to balance by weighting in performance predicting training data (p 129) <but this doesn’t seem fair> This procedure can be repeated any number of times, allowing statistical tests

10 Comparing Data Mining Methods Using T-Test
Don’t worry about the math You may not have had it WEKA will do it automatically for you – experimenter environment Excel can do it easily See examplettest.xls file on my www site (formular =TTEST(A1:A8,B1:B8,2,1) two ranges being compared two-tailed test, since we don’t know which to expect to be higher 1 – indicates paired test – ok when results being compared are from th same samples (same splits into folds) result is probability that differences are not chance generally accepted if below .05 but sometimes looking for .01 or better

11 5.6 Predicting Probabilities
Skip

12 5.7 Counting the Cost Some mistakes are more costly to make than others Giving a loan to a defaulter is more costly than denying somebody who would be a good customer Sending mail solicitation to somebody who won’t buy is less costly than missing somebody who would buy (opportunity cost) Looking at a confusion matrix, each position could have an associated cost (or benefit from correct positions) Measurement could be average profit/ loss per prediction To be fair in cost benefit analysis, should also factor in cost of collecting and preparing the data, building the model …

13 Lift Charts In practice, costs are frequently not known
Decisions may be made by comparing possible scenarios Book Example – Promotional Mailing Situation 1 – previous experience predicts that 0.1% of all (1,000,000) households will respond Situation 2 – classifier predicts that 0.4% of the most promising households will respond Situation 3 – classifier predicts that 0.2% of the most promising households will respond The increase in response rate is the lift ( 0.4 / 0.1 = 4 in situation 2 compared to sending to all) A lift chart allows for a visual comparison …

14 Figure 5.1 A hypothetical lift chart.

15 Generating a lift chart
Best done if classifier generates probabilities for its predictions Sort test instances based on probability of class we’re interested in (e.g. would buy from catalog = yes) Table 5.6 Rank Probability of Yes Actual class 1 .95 Yes 2 .93 3 No 4 .88 5 .86 Yes … to get y-value (# correct) for a given x (sample size), read down sorted list to sample size, counting number of instances that are actually the class we want (e.g. sample size = 5, correct = 4 – on lift chart shown, the sample size of 5 would be converted to % or total sample)

16 Cost Sensitive Classification
For classifiers that generate probabilities of each class If not cost sensitive, would predict most probable class With costs shown, and probabilities A=.2 B= .3 C= .5 Expected Costs of Predictions = A  .2 * * * 10 = 6.5 B  .2 * * * 2 = 3.0 C  .2 * * * 0 = 5.5 Considering costs, B would be predicted even though C is considered most likely Costs Predicted A B C Actual 10 20 5 2

17 Cost Sensitive Learning
Most learning methods are not sensitive to cost structures (e.g. higher cost of false positive than false negative) (Naïve Bayes is, decision tree learners not) Simple method for making cost sensitive – Change proportion of different classes in the data E.g. if have a dataset with 1000 yes, and 1000 no, but incorrectly predicting Yes is 10 times more costly than incorrectly predicting No Filter and sample the data so that have 1000 No and 100 Yes A learning scheme trying to minimize errors is going to tend toward predicting No If don’t have enough data to put some aside, “re-sample” No’s (bring duplicates in) (if learning method can deal with duplicates (most can)) With some methods, you can “Weight” instances so that some count more than others. No’s could be more heavily weighted

18 Information Retrieval (IR) Measures
E.g., Given a WWW search, a search engine produces a list of hits supposedly relevant Which is better? Retrieving 100, of which 40 are actually relevant Retrieving 400, of which 80 are actually relevant Really depends on the costs

19 Information Retrieval (IR) Measures
IR community has developed 3 measures: Recall = number of documents retrieved that are relevant total number of documents that are relevant Precision = number of documents retrieved that are relevant total number of documents that are retrieved F-measure = 2 * recall * precision recall + precision … coverage of yes’s (in this example) … proportion of yes’s (in this example) Attempt to balance the two **** Recall and precision generally trade off – the more documents retrieved, (unless our method doesn’t work), the higher the recall, but probably the lower the precision – as more documents are retrieved we will be returning more that we are less convinced about, and will likely get increasing numbers of documents that are not relevant. Assuming that this trade off occurs, F-measure is maximized if both recall and precision are in the middle, and gets lower as one is increased at the expense of the other e.g. 2 * ½ * ½ / ½ + ½ = .5 2 * ¼ * ¾ / ¼ + ¾ = .375 2 * 1/10 * 9/10 / 1/10 + 9/10 = .18 …

20 WEKA TP rate and recall are the same = TP / (TP + FN)
Part of the results provided by WEKA (that we’ve ignored so far) Let’s look at an example (Naïve Bayes on my-weather-nominal) === Detailed Accuracy By Class === TP Rate FP Rate Precision Recall F-Measure Class yes no === Confusion Matrix === a b <-- classified as 4 2 | a = yes 1 7 | b = no TP rate and recall are the same = TP / (TP + FN) For Yes = 4 / (4 + 2); For No = 7 / (7 + 1) FP rate = FP / (FP + TN) – For Yes = 1 / (1 + 7); For No = 2 / (2 + 4) Precision = TP / (TP + FP) – For yes = 4 / (4 + 1); For No = 7 / (7 + 2) F-measure = 2TP / (2TP + FP + FN) For Yes = 2*4 / (2* ) = 8 / 11 For No = 2 * 7 / (2* ) = 14/17

21 In terms of true positives etc
True positives = TP; False positives = FP True Negatives = TN; False negatives = FN Recall = TP / (TP + FN) // true positives / actually positive Precision = TP / (TP + FP) // true positives / predicted positive F-measure = 2TP / (2TP + FP + FN) This has been generated using algebra from the formula previous Easier to understand this way – correct predictions are double counted – once for recall, once for precision. denominator includes corrects and incorrects (either based on recall or precision idea – relevant but not retrieved or retrieved but not relevant) There is no mathematics that says recall and precision can be combined this way – it is ad hoc – but it does balance the two

22 Kappa Statistic A way of checking success against how hard the problem is Compare to expected results from random prediction … with predictions in the same proportion as the predictions made by the classifier being evaluated This is different than predicting in proportion to the actual values Which might be considered having an unfair advantage But which I would consider a better measure

23 Kappa Statistic A B C Total 88 10 2 100 14 40 6 60 18 12 120 20 A B C
Predicted Predicted A B C Total 88 10 2 100 14 40 6 60 18 12 120 20 A B C Total 60 30 10 100 36 18 6 24 12 4 40 120 20 A C T U L The idea is to compare actual results with what would have happened if a random predictor predicted answers in the same proportion that the actual predictor did On the left is the actual results – with = 140 correct out of 200 (70%) The actual proportions are 100, 60, and 40 for A,B,C The prediction proportions are 120, 60, 20 for A,B, and C On the right. The prediction proportions are matched – but predictions are random so those 120 predictions of A are split among all of the actual values since 50% of the actual answers are A (100 of 200), of those 120 predictions of A we expect half to actually be correct (60) Since 30% of actual answers are B (60 of 200), of the 120 predictions of A, we expect 30% to be for instances that are actually Bs (36) Since 20% of actual answers are C (40 of 200), of the 120 predictions of A, we expect 20% to be for instances that are actually Cs (24) We’re going to have 60 predictions of B, but predictions are random so we expect those predictions to be split among all of the actual values since 50% of the actual answers are A (100 of 200), of those 60 predictions of B we expect half to be for instances that are actually As (30) Since 30% of actual answers are B (60 of 200), of the 60 predictions of B, we expect 30% to be correct (18) Since 20% of actual answers are C (40 of 200), of the 60 predictions of B, we expect 20% to be for instances that are actually Cs (12) We’re going to have 20 predictions of C, but predictions are random so we expect those predictions to be split among all of the actual values since 50% of the actual answers are A (100 of 200), of those 20 predictions of C we expect half to be for instances that are actually As (10) Since 30% of actual answers are B (60 of 200), of the 20 predictions of C, we expect 30% to be for instances that are actually Bs (6) Since 20% of actual answers are C (40 of 200), of the 20 predictions of C, we expect 20% to be correct (4) The expected results with stratified random prediction is =82 So this classifier being tested squeezed an extra = 58 correct predictions The best possible classifier (100% correct) could have obtained an extra =118 correct predictions So our classifier got 58 out of possible 118 extra correct predictions (49.2%)  that’s the Kappa Statistic <it’s not cost sensitive, so I’m not sure why it’s in the cost sensitive section> Actual Results Expected Results with Stratified Random Prediction

24 WEKA For many occasions, this borders on “too much information”, but it’s all there We can decide, are we more interested in Yes , or No? Are we more interested in recall or precision?

25 WEKA – with more than two classes
Contact Lenses with Naïve Bayes === Detailed Accuracy By Class === TP Rate FP Rate Precision Recall F-Measure Class soft hard none === Confusion Matrix === a b c <-- classified as | a = soft | b = hard | c = none Class exercise – show how to calculate recall, precision, f-measure for each class

26 Evaluating Numeric Prediction
Here, not a matter of right or wrong, but rather, “how far off” There are a number of possible measures, with formulas shown in Table 5.6

27 WEKA IBK w/ k = 5 on baskball.arff === Cross-validation ===
=== Summary === Correlation coefficient Mean absolute error Root mean squared error Relative absolute error % Root relative squared error % Total Number of Instances

28 Root Mean-Squared Error
Squareroot of (Sum of Squares of Errors / number of predictions) Algorithm: Initialize – especially subtotal = 0 Loop through all test instances Make prediction, compare to actual – calculate difference Square difference; add to subtotal Divide subtotal by number of test instances Take squareroot to obtain root mean squared error Error is on same scale as predictions – root mean squared error can be compared to mean of .42 and a range of .67, seems decent Exaggerates effect of any bad predictions, since differences are squared

29 Mean Absolute Error (Sum of Absolute Values of Errors / number of predictions) Algorithm: Initialize – especially subtotal = 0 Loop through all test instances Make prediction, compare to actual – calculate difference Take absolute value of difference; add to subtotal Divide subtotal by number of test instances to obtain mean absolute error Error is on same scale as predictions –mean absolute error can be compared to mean of .42 and a range of .67, seems decent Does not exaggerate the effect of any bad predictions, NOTE – this value is smaller in my example than the squared version.

30 Relative Error Measures
Results are divided by differences from mean Root Relative Squared Error Relative Absolute Error See upcoming slides

31 Root Relative Squared Error
Squareroot of (Sum of Squares of Errors / Sum of Squares of differences from mean) Gives idea of scale of error compared to how variable the actual values are (the more variable the values are, really the harder the task) Algorithm: Initialize – especially numerator and denominator subtotals = 0 Determine mean of actual test instances Loop through all test instances Make prediction, compare to actual – calculate difference Square difference; add to numerator subtotal Compare actual to mean of actual – calculate difference Square difference; add to denominator subtotal Divide numerator subtotal by denominator subtotal Take squareroot of above result to obtain root relative squared error Error is nornalized Use of squares once again exaggerates Error is nornalized – relative absolute error (83%) tells us that are predictions are not as variable from the actual as the actual is from the mean, so we are getting somewhere, but not really that fast Use of squares once again exaggerates effect of any bad predictions; here we also exaggerate the effect (on denominator) of unusual test instances

32 Relative Absolute Error
Sum of Absolute Values of Errors / Sum of Absolute Values of differences from mean) Gives idea of scale of error compared to how variable the actual values are (the more variable the values are, really the harder the task) Algorithm: Initialize – especially numerator and denominator subtotals = 0 Determine mean of actual test instances Loop through all test instances Make prediction, compare to actual – calculate difference; take absolute value of difference; add to numerator subtotal Compare actual to mean of actual – calculate difference take absolute value of difference; add to denominator subtotal Divide numerator subtotal by denominator subtotal Error is nornalized Does not exaggerate <sounds like an oxymoron> Error is nornalized – root relative squared error (85%) tells us that are predictions are not as variable from the actual as the actual is from the mean, so we are getting somewhere, but not really that fast any bad predictions are not exaggerated in their effects; unusual test instances do not have exaggerated effect on the denominator

33 Correlation Coefficient
Tells whether the predictions and actual values “move together” – one goes up when the other goes up … Not as tight a measurement as others E.g. if predictions are all double the actual, correlation is perfect 1.0, but predictions are not that good We want to have a good correlation, but we want MORE than that A little bit complicated, and well established (can do easily in Excel), so let’s skip the math

34 What to use? Depends some on philosophy
Do you want to punish bad predictions a lot? (then use a root squared method) Do you want to compare performance on different data sets and one might be “harder” (more variable) than another? (then use a relative method) In many real world cases, any of these work fine (comparisons between algorithms come out the same regardless of which measurement is used) Basic framework same as with predicting category – repeated 10-fold cross validation, with paired sampling …

35 Minimum Description Length Principle
What is learned in Data Mining is a form of “theory” Occam’s Razor – in science, others things being equal, simple theories are preferable to complex ones Mistakes a theory makes, really are exceptions, so to keep other things equal they should be added to the theory, making it more complex Simple example  a simple decision tree (other things being equal) is preferred over a more complex decision tree Details will be skipped (along with section 5.10)

36 End Chapter 5


Download ppt "Data Mining – Credibility: Evaluating What’s Been Learned"

Similar presentations


Ads by Google