Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Lecture 1: Fundamentals of epidemiologic study design and analysis

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Lecture 1: Fundamentals of epidemiologic study design and analysis"— Presentation transcript:

1 Lecture 1: Fundamentals of epidemiologic study design and analysis
Jeffrey E. Korte, PhD BMTRY 747: Foundations of Epidemiology II Department of Public Health Sciences Medical University of South Carolina Spring 2015

2 Basic study designs Ecologic Cohort Case-control
Cross-sectional Longitudinal Cohort Case-control Randomized controlled trial

3 Other study designs Case-cohort Nested case-control Case-crossover

4 Ecologic studies Unit of observation: geographical area
No individual information available Analyze correlations between: Mean value of exposure of interest Rate of disease of interest Vulnerable to “ecologic fallacy”

5 Ecologic fallacy “Marginal” information known
Individual information not known Exp Not exp Dis ? 50 No dis 950 200 800 1000

6 Ecologic fallacy (exposure appears related to disease)
Not exp Dis ? 50 No dis 950 200 800 1000 Exp Not exp Dis ? 150 No dis 850 500 1000 Region 1 Region 2

7 Ecologic fallacy unmasked (there is actually no association)
Exp Not exp Dis 10 40 50 No dis 190 760 950 200 800 1000 Exp Not exp Dis 75 150 No dis 425 850 500 1000 Region 1 Region 2

8 Ecologic fallacy Scenario 2: example from Szklo book, page 16
Are poor people bad drivers??? (see next slide)

9

10 Ecologic studies Great for generating hypotheses
Aggregate measures (mean across people) Environmental (physical exposures) Global measures (sociopolitical, etc.) e.g. dietary fat intake/breast cancer; vitamin D/prostate cancer Mixed individual-ecologic study Some variables are measured using an ecologic criterion (neighborhood characteristics, etc.)

11 Cross-sectional studies
Single timepoint May be baseline data from a cohort study Assess association between exposure and disease of interest Limited to prevalent disease outcomes Reflects incidence rate and duration/survival Exposure data more vulnerable to recall bias Less time-consuming, less expensive

12 Cross-sectional studies
Disadvantage: concurrent exposure and disease information restricts causal inference May be able to collect historical exposure data in questionnaire (vulnerable to recall bias)

13 Cohort studies Assemble individuals without disease
Assess exposure status Follow individuals over time Observe incident disease events Avoid recall bias More expensive, time-consuming

14 Cohort studies Can estimate disease risk, disease rate
Can estimate proportion exposed (if population-based sample) Can evaluate numerous outcomes Can evaluate numerous exposures Cohort study can be basis for more efficient study designs (case-cohort, nested case-control)

15 Cohort studies Occupational exposures:
Can use occupational cohorts to evaluate specific exposures (e.g. chemicals, radiation) at high doses in humans Not representative of general population Vulnerable to healthy worker survival effect Exposed group and comparison group may have comparability problems

16 Cohort studies Retrospective cohort studies
Historical exposure data is available Medical records are available through time Cohort is assembled and followed through historical time to simulate a prospective cohort study Often used in occupational studies Less expensive and time-consuming

17 Case-control studies Individuals recruited into study based on disease status Case definition can be critical Historical exposure information obtained Exposure compared between cases and controls Vulnerable to recall bias

18 Case-control studies Selection of control group is critical
Population-based, hospital-based? Matching? Controls should be representative of the population from which the cases occurred (people who would have been recruited as cases if they had had the disease of interest)

19 Case-control studies See Figure 1-19, Szklo page 26: survival bias
(see next slide)

20

21 Case-cohort study Based in cohort study
Sub-cohort is identified: subset of participants at baseline Sub-cohort may include eventual cases Individuals who become cases are compared to sub-cohort

22 Case-cohort study Advantages:
less expensive than cohort study, if lab tests are done on selected stored samples instead of all samples Exposures assessed before incident disease (no recall bias) Sub-cohort can be comparison group for more than one case group (e.g. different disease)

23 Case-cohort study See Figure 1-21, Szklo page 28 (see next slide)

24

25 Nested case-control study
Based in cohort study As cases arise, one or more (matched) controls are selected Individuals may serve as a control at one timepoint, then serve as a case at a later timepoint Advantages similar to case-cohort design

26 Nested case-control study
See Figure 1-20, page 27 Szklo (see next slide)

27

28 Case-crossover design
All individuals have the disease of interest Exposure for each individual is compared at one timepoint (e.g. just before diagnosis) versus another timepoint (e.g. one year earlier) Useful for acute effects of exposure (e.g. environmental, psychological, physical)

29 Randomized controlled trials
Experiment May test medical, behavioral, social intervention Compare outcomes between groups Randomization should eliminate the possibility of bias or confounding, even from unknown confounders Findings may not be generalizable, depending on sampling strategy/recruitment into study

30 Measures and associations
Strength of association Risk ratio, odds ratio, hazard ratio, rate ratio 1.0 denotes no association (i.e. the exposure groups have the same risk) Statistical significance / p value Chi-square, t-test, multivariable regression p<0.05 denotes statistical significance Confidence intervals Show precision of estimate and statistical significance

31 Measures and Associations (continuous outcome)
Mean Median Percentiles Difference between means Can calculate risk ratio for 1-unit increase, 10-unit increase, etc. (association is assumed to be constant over the exposure range)

32 Measures and Associations (categorical outcome)
Prevalence Incidence Risk Odds Relative risk / risk ratio Hazard ratio Odds ratio Rate ratio

33 Prevalence People currently living with a health outcome of interest (e.g. 72%, 137/100,000, etc.) Prevalence is a reflection of several factors Incidence rate Cure rate Progression rate Death rate Explanatory factors (e.g. age, causal exposures, medical care)

34 Prevalence Point prevalence Period prevalence Lifetime prevalence

35 Incidence New cases of disease (risk or rate)
Occur over time, during study follow-up or during public health surveillance Reflects: Changes in diagnostic standards Screening bias (early detection) Latent period (undetected/undetectable) (these factors also affect prevalence)

36 Risk Same as “proportion”
Assumes all individuals have the same follow-up time Risk ratio: disease risk in exposed group, divided by risk in unexposed group

37 Rate Individuals do not need to have the same follow-up time
Denominator is person-years of follow-up in each group Sum of individual follow-up times Rate ratio: disease rate in exposed group, divided by rate in unexposed group

38 Rate (example) Deaths Person-yrs Rate Group 1 45 13,739 32.8/10,000
15,180 21.1/10,000 relative rate = 1.6

39 Risk Ratio vs. Rate Ratio
Example: compare two samples followed over 5 years. Group 1: 20% develop cancer, all within the first year Group 2: 20% develop cancer, all during Year 5 What is the risk ratio? What about the rate ratio?

40 Odds Probability of having disease (or exposure), divided by probability of not having disease (or exposure) Useful for case-control study Odds ratio is a good estimate of risk ratio for rare diseases

41 Odds ratio Exp No exp Dis 45 5 50 No dis 10 40 55 100
Odds of exposure in cases: 45/5 Odds of exposure in controls: 10/40 Exposure odds ratio = (45/5)/(10/40) = 36 Disease odds ratio also equals 36

42 Risk ratio Exp No exp Dis 45 5 50 No dis 10 40 55 100
Risk of disease in exposed: 45/55 Risk of disease in unexposed: 5/45 Risk ratio = (45/55)/(5/45) = 7.4

43 Risk ratio Exp No exp Dis 9 1 10 No dis 250 740 990 259 741 1000
Risk of disease in exposed: 9/259 Risk of disease in unexposed: 1/741 Risk ratio = (9/259)/(1/741) = 25.7

44 Hazard ratio Used in survival analysis (Cox proportional hazards model) of cohort studies “Time-to-event” information is the outcome of interest When each case arises, all other noncensored, healthy individuals serve as controls for that case at that timepoint. The model assumes that the hazard ratio (exposed/unexposed) stays constant over time.

45 Rate ratio Can be estimated in Poisson regression
Another generalized linear model Uses “count” data, typically in cohort studies The model assumes that independent risk factors result in multiplicative risks.

46 Risk difference Can be estimated in linear regression
Outcome is assumed to be continuous, normally distributed Exposures can be continuous, ordinal, or categorical The model assumes that independent risk factors result in additive risks.

47 The Golden Rule Thou shalt design and analyze epidemiologic studies in such a way as to allow you to answer the scientific question of interest. Corollary: methods are a means to an end. Implication: do not adapt the question to fit the methods.


Download ppt "Lecture 1: Fundamentals of epidemiologic study design and analysis"

Similar presentations


Ads by Google