Download presentation
Presentation is loading. Please wait.
1
Comparative Models of Classical Conditioning
Modern Twists on Rescorla-Wagner
2
Assumptions of Rescorla-Wagner (1974) model
Model developed to accurately predict and map learning as it occurs trial by trial Assumes a bunch of givens: Assume animal can perceive CS and US, and can exhibit UR and CR Helpful for the animal to know 2 things about conditioning: what TYPE of event is coming the SIZE of the upcoming event Thus, classical conditioning is really learning about: signals (CS's) which are PREDICTORS for important events (US's)
3
Assumptions of R-W model
Assumes that with each CS-US pairing 1 of 3 things can happen: The CS might become more INHIBITORY The CS might become more EXCITATORY There is no change in the CS How do these 3 rules work? If US is larger than expected: CS = excitatory If US is smaller than expected: CS= inhibitory If US = expectations: No change in CS The effect of reinforcers or nonreinforcers on the change of associative strength depends upon: The existing associative strength of THAT CS AND on the associative strength of other stimuli concurrently present
4
Explanation of how an animal anticipates what type of CS is coming:
Direct link is assumed between "CS center" and "US center": E.g. between a tone center and food center In 1970’s: other researchers thought R and W were crazy with this idea Now: neuroscience shows formation of neural circuits! Assumes that STRENGTH of an event is given The conditioning situation is predicted by the strength of the learned connection THUS: when learning is complete: The strength of the association relates directly to the size or intensity of the CS Asymptote of learning = max learning that can occur to that size or intensity of a CS Maximum amount of learning that a given CS can support
5
More assumptions The change in associative strength of a CS as the result of any given trial can be predicted from the composite strength resulting from all stimuli presented on that trial: Composite strength = summation of conditioning that occurs to all stimuli present during a conditioning trial If composite strength is LOW: the ability of reinforcer to produce increments in the strength of component stimuli is HIGH More can be learned for this trial If the composite strength is HIGH: reinforcement is relatively less effective (LOW) Less can be learned for this trial- approaching max of learning
6
WHY is this equation important?
We can use the three rules to make predictions about amount and direction of classical conditioning λ j > Vsum = Excitatory Conditioning The degree to which the CS predicted the size of the US was GREATER than expected, so you react MORE to the CS next trial λ j < Vsum = Inhibitory Conditioning The degree to which the CS predicted the size of the US was LESS than expected, so you react LESS to the CS next trial λ j = Vsum = no change: The CS predicted the size of the US exactly as you expected
7
Can also explain why probability of reward given CS vs no CS makes a difference:
π = probability of US given the CS or No US given No CS Are 3 basic rules: If p(US|CS)>p(US|noCS): πax > πa then Vx should be POSITIVE If p(US|CS)<p(US|noCS): πax < πa then Vx should be NEGATIVE If p(US|CS)=p(US|noCS): πax = πa then Vx should be ZERO Vi = πaxαißj(Λj-Vsum)
8
Explaining loss of Associate Value despite pairings with the US:
Is an unusual prediction: Associative value of a CS can decrease despite continued pairings with the US Show this with three-phase experiment: Phase 1: CSA US (1 food pellet) and CSB US (I food pellet on separate trials (equal #) Phase 2: CSA and CSB paired together 1 food pellet Same US, so same amount of conditioning, right?? RW model predicts that conditional properties of A and B individually will DECREASE in phase 2.
9
Explaining loss of Associate Value despite pairings with the US:
Why: Overexpectation VA= λ ; VB= λ for phase 1; VA+B = 2 λ This is an over-expectation effect (we will see this in operant, too!) Got 1 pellet with A Got 1 pellet with B So how many pellets when A+B? Not get 2 pellets, so Phase 2 = decremental conditioning
10
Conditioned Inhibition
RW model also predicts Conditioned inhibition Can test for Conditioned inhibition 2 kinds of trials CS+US trials CS- no US trials Consider 2 kinds of trials separately Reinforced trials: CS+ gains excitatory properties Unreinforced trials: CS- gains inhibitory properties When presented together: cancel one another out (assuming equal conditioning) Is a SUMMATIVE effect
11
Extinction of excitation and inhibition
During extinction: EXT: CS+ no US Initially: animal expects CS-US and is excitatory This excitatory expectation is quickly diminished with repeated trials of no US Decreases the excitatory conditioning incrementally, just like the excitatory conditioning was increased incrementally during learning
12
Dopamine and Rescorla Wagner Model
Turns out that changes in dopamine (DA) levels in dorsal striatal limbic cortical pathway vary as we learn And guess what: these levels can be predicted by the RW model! But, once a CS-US pairing (or an operant R-SR pairing) become well learned, the circuit begins to involve lower parts of the brain Circuit begins to involve basal striatal areas Becomes an “automated” or mastered behavior No longer involves being “surprised” Only returns to this pathway if the CS-US relation change!!!!!
13
Critique of the Rescorla-Wagner Model:
R-W model really a theory about the US effectiveness: Says little about CS effectiveness Doesn’t really deal with how CS effectiveness may vary How WELL a CS predicts as a combo of salience and probability but doesn’t really evaluate this States that an unpredicted US is effective in promoting learning, whereas a well-predicted US is ineffective because nothing is left to learn.
14
Critique of the Rescorla-Wagner Model:
Fails to predict the CS-pre-exposure effect: Two groups of subjects Grp I CS-US pairings Control Grp II CS alone CS-US pairings PRE-Expos Bob and Tom effect Bob always hangs with Tom You are dating Tom You have a BAAAAAD breakup with Tom Now you hate Bob….why? Original model didn’t address how exposure could change salience/learning to CS Incidental “CS” exposure (before it was a CS) can affect its salience and learn-ability We learn about stimuli even if they aren’t predictive We learn they AREN’T predictive We learn “who they hang around”…..stimuli that group together
15
Critique of the Rescorla-Wagner Model:
In pre-exposure effect, simply being around a neutral stimulus alters its ability to become conditioned Original R-W model doesn't predict any difference, Assumes no conditioning trials occur when CSs presented in absence of US so Vsum = 0 This appears to be wrong Conditioning likely occurring any time 2 stimuli are together Form an incidental association Need to modify the equation to account for this They have, but we won’t!
16
Critique of the Rescorla-Wagner Model:
Original R-W model implies that salience is fixed for any given CS R-W assume CS salience doesn't change w/experience More recent data strongly suggest CS salience DOES change w/experience Research shows CS salience CHANGES! Why changes in CS salience? Data suggest that Salience to a CS DECREASES when CS is repeatedly presented without consequence CS that is accidentally paired with another CS INCREASES in salience Thus: Appears that CS and US effectiveness are both highly important As a result of these types of data: Given birth to attentional models of CC
17
Three Primary Extensions and Alternatives to RW model
Attentional models Timing and Information models Comparator hypothesis
18
Attentional Models of CC
Question: How well does the CS command attention? Assumes that increased attention facilitates learning about a stimulus Procedures that disrupt attention to CS disrupt learning Different attentional models differ in assumptions about what determines how much attention a CS commands on any given trial Single attentional mechanisms: Kamin’s surprise Multiple attentional mechanisms: Current models examine changes in multiple types of attention
19
Multiple attentional mechanisms:
Three attentions: Looking for action: attention a CS commands after it has become a good predictor of the CS Looking for learning: how well the organism processes cues that are not yet good predictors of the US, and thus have to be “learned about” Looking for liking: the emotional/affective properties of the CS Assume that the outcome of a given trial alters the degree of attention commanded by the CS on future trials Surprise? Then an increase in looking for learning on next trial Good predictor? Then increase in attention on next trial Pleasant outcome? Increases emotional value of CS on next trial
20
Timing/Information Theory Models
Recognized that time is important factor in CC: Timberlake, et al 1980 Focal search Responses become conditioned when CS-US interval is short Short CS-US intervals condition responses congruent with a focal search Immediate contact with the CS; food preparation responses General search Different responses become conditioned when CS-US interval is long Long CS-US intervals condition responses congruent with a general search Longer intervals result in general search responses such as looking, searching, etc. Suggests that organisms learn both WHAT is predicted HOW MUCH is predicted WHEN what is predicted will occur.
21
Temporal coding hypothesis
Organisms learn when the US occurs in relation to the CS Use this information in blocking, second-order conditioning, etc. Not pay attention to the tone, because it occurs with the light If the tone were to occur before/after the light, then different CS situation What is learned in one phase of training influences what is learned in subsequent phase Overshadowing Pre-exposure effect Blocking Large literature supports this
22
Importance of Inter-trial interval
More conditioned responding observed with a longer inter-trial interval Inter-trial interval is the time BETWEEN trials If very short; less learning If longer; more learning BUT: Intertrial interval and CS duration (CS-US interval) act in combination to determine responding Critical factor: Relative duration of these two temporal intervals rather than absolute value of either one by itself Need quick CS-US pairing (delayed conditioning) then a longer inter-trial interval Why?
23
Importance of Inter-trial interval
Holland (2000) Conditioned rats to an auditory cue that was presented just before delivery to food CR to CS: Nosing of food cup (goal tracking) Each group conditioned with: 1 of 2 CS durations: 10 or 20 sec 1 of 6 intertrial intervals: 15 to 960 sec Characterized responses in terms of the ratio of the intertrial interval (I) and the CS duration (T). Time spent nosing the food cup during CS plotted as function of relative value of I/T Results: as I/T ratio increases, the percentage of time the rats spend with the nose in the food cup increases
24
Why is the Inter-trial interval so important?
Relative Waiting Time Hypothesis Organism is making a comparison between events during the Intertrial interval (I) and Trial interval (T) How long one has to wait for the US during the CS vs. How long one has to wait for the US during the intertrial interval When US waiting time during CS is shorter than intertrial interval: I/T ratio is high CS is highly informative about the next occurrence of the US Lots of responding When US waiting time during CS is same/longer than intertrial interval wait: I/T ratio is low CS is not highly informative Less responding
25
Comparitor Hypothesis
Ralph Miller, et al.: Organism compares across learning situations Assumes: Conditional responding depends on What happens during CS ALSO what happens in other aspects of experimental situation (when CS is not presented) That is, animal compares presence of CS to absence of CS Revaluation effects: Can better explain blocking What is blocked is RESPONDING to CSAB, not learning of CSAB Can unmask blocking to CSAB by presenting CSA alone without US (EXT CSA). Changes conditional value of CSA Now CSAB has different, predictive value Animal “revalues” CSAB and now responds as it is predictive, whereas CSA is not.
26
Comparator Hypothesis
Note: This is a theory of PERFORMANCE, not learning! Assumes conditioned responding depends on BOTH associations between CS-US and between US and other stimuli when CS not there These other stimuli form the “comparator” cues Also assumes formation of excitatory associations with US ONLY Instead of forming inhibitory associations with CS, form stronger excitatory associations with “not CS” Animal compares the relative associative value of “CS vs. “not CS” Whatever is stronger is what you respond to
27
Why are these theories important?
Helps explain the PROCESS of classical conditioning Describes conditions and allows predictions of Who is likely to form associations between stimuli To What kinds of stimuli it will occur When classical conditioning will occur Where associations form: what settings/conditions are important How well: how fast/slow, what quality of associations are formed. Important for predicting/controlling behavior in applied settings Understanding and treatment of pedophilia, sexual fetishes, etc. Phobia treatment Commercials and advertising Social behaviors
Similar presentations
© 2024 SlidePlayer.com. Inc.
All rights reserved.