Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Chapter Thirteen: Alternative Research Designs

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Chapter Thirteen: Alternative Research Designs"— Presentation transcript:

1 Chapter Thirteen: Alternative Research Designs
The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

2 Protecting Internal Validity Revisited
A type of evaluation of your experiment. It asks the question of whether your IV is the only possible explanation of the results shown for your DV. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

3 Protecting Internal Validity Revisited
Confounding Confounding is caused by an uncontrolled extraneous variable that varies systematically with the IV. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

4 Protecting Internal Validity Revisited
Extraneous Variables Extraneous variables are variables that may unintentionally operate to influence the dependent variable. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

5 Protecting Internal Validity Revisited
Cause-and-Effect Relation A cause-and-effect relation occurs when we know that a particular IV (cause) leads to specific changes in a DV (effect). The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

6 Protecting Internal Validity Revisited
Internal validity revolves around the question of whether your IV actually created any change that you observe in your DV. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

7 Protecting Internal Validity Revisited
Internal validity revolves around the question of whether your IV actually created any change that you observe in your DV. When you have an internally valid experiment, you are reasonably certain that your IV is responsible for the changes you observed in your DV. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

8 Protecting Internal Validity Revisited
Good experimental control leads to internally valid experiments. See toothpaste test p of your text. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

9 Protecting Internal Validity with Research Designs
Experimental design recommendations from Campbell (1957) and Campbell and Stanley (1966): The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

10 Protecting Internal Validity with Research Designs
Experimental design recommendations from Campbell (1957) and Campbell and Stanley (1966): Random Assignment Experimental participants are distributed into various groups on a random (nonsystematic) basis. All participants have an equal chance of being assigned to any of our treatment groups. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

11 Protecting Internal Validity with Research Designs
Experimental design recommendations from Campbell (1957) and Campbell and Stanley (1966): Random Assignment Experimental participants are distributed into various groups on a random (nonsystematic) basis. All participants have an equal chance of being assigned to any of our treatment groups. The only drawback to random assignment is that we cannot guarantee equality through its use. Random assignment is not the same as random selection (choosing participants from a population in such a way that all possible participants have an equal opportunity to be chosen. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

12 Protecting Internal Validity with Research Designs
Campbell and Stanley (1966) recommended three experimental designs as being able to control the threats to internal validity we listed in Chapter 6. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

13 Protecting Internal Validity with Research Designs
Campbell and Stanley (1966) recommended three experimental designs as being able to control the threats to internal validity we listed in Chapter 6. These designs are: The Pretest-Posttest Control Group Design The Solomon Four-Group Design The Posttest-Only Control-Group Design The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

14 The Pretest-Posttest Control Group Design
This design consists of two randomly assigned groups of participants, both of which are pretested, with one group receiving the IV. The random assignment of participants to groups allows us to assume that the two groups are equated before the experiment, thus ruling out selection as a problem. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

15 The Pretest-Posttest Control Group Design
Selection A threat to internal validity. If we choose participants in such a way that our groups are not equal before the experiment, we cannot be certain that our IV caused any difference we observe after the experiment. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

16 The Pretest-Posttest Control Group Design
Using a pretest and posttest for both groups allows us to control the effects of history, maturation, and testing because they should affect both groups equally. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

17 The Pretest-Posttest Control Group Design
Using a pretest and posttest for both groups allows us to control the effects of history, maturation, and testing because they should affect both groups equally. If the control group shows a change between the pretests and posttests, then we know that some factor other than the IV is at work. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

18 The Pretest-Posttest Control Group Design
Using a pretest and posttest for both groups allows us to control the effects of history, maturation, and testing because they should affect both groups equally. If the control group shows a change between the pretests and posttests, then we know that some factor other than the IV is at work. Statistical regression is controlled as long as we assign our experimental and control groups from the same extreme pool of participants. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

19 The Pretest-Posttest Control Group Design
Using a pretest and posttest for both groups allows us to control the effects of history, maturation, and testing because they should affect both groups equally. If the control group shows a change between the pretests and posttests, then we know that some factor other than the IV is at work. Statistical regression is controlled as long as we assign our experimental and control groups from the same extreme pool of participants. If any of the interactions with selection occur, they should affect both groups equally, thus equalizing those effects on internal validity. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

20 The Pretest-Posttest Control Group Design
History A threat to internal validity. History refers to events that occur between the DV measurements in a repeated measures design. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

21 The Pretest-Posttest Control Group Design
Maturation A threat to internal validity. Maturation refers to changes in participants that occur over time during an experiment. These changes could include actual physical maturation or tiredness, boredom, hunger, and so on. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

22 The Pretest-Posttest Control Group Design
Testing A threat to internal validity that occurs because measuring the DV causes changes in the DV. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

23 The Pretest-Posttest Control Group Design
Statistical Regression A threat to internal validity that occurs when low scorers improve or high scorers fall on a second administration of a test due solely to statistical reasons. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

24 The Pretest-Posttest Control Group Design
Interactions with Selection Threats to internal validity that can occur if there are systematic differences between or among selected treatment groups based on maturation, history, or instrumentation. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

25 The Pretest-Posttest Control Group Design
Instrumentation A threat to internal validity that occurs if the equipment or human measuring the DV changes its measuring criterion over time. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

26 The Pretest-Posttest Control Group Design
Diffusion or Tmitation of Treatment A threat to internal validity that can occur if participants in one treatment group become familiar with the treatment of another group and copy that treatment. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

27 The Solomon Four-Group Design
This design is identical to the pretest-posttest control-group design with the first two groups but adds an additional two groups. Because the Solomon four-group design has the same two groups as the pretest-posttest control-group design, it has the same protection against the threats to internal validity. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

28 The Solomon Four-Group Design
This design is identical to the pretest-posttest control-group design with the first two groups but adds an additional two groups. Because the Solomon four-group design has the same two groups as the pretest-posttest control-group design, it has the same protection against the threats to internal validity. The main advantage gained by adding the two additional groups relates to external validity. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

29 The Solomon Four-Group Design
This design is identical to the pretest-posttest control-group design with the first two groups but adds an additional two groups. Because the Solomon four-group design has the same two groups as the pretest-posttest control-group design, it has the same protection against the threats to internal validity. The main advantage gained by adding the two additional groups relates to external validity. One problem with the Solomon design is that there is no statistical test that can treat all six sets of data at the same time. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

30 The Posttest-Only Control-Group Design
The posttest-only control-group design is a copy of the pretest-posttest control-group design but without the pretests included and is a copy of the two added groups in the Solomon four-group design. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

31 The Posttest-Only Control-Group Design
The posttest-only control-group design is a copy of the pretest-posttest control-group design but without the pretests included and is a copy of the two added groups in the Solomon four-group design. Random assignment to groups equates the two groups and withholding the IV from one group to make it a control group makes it a powerful experimental design that covers the threats to internal validity. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

32 The Posttest-Only Control-Group Design
The posttest-only control-group design is a copy of the pretest-posttest control-group design but without the pretests included and is a copy of the two added groups in the Solomon four-group design. Random assignment to groups equates the two groups and withholding the IV from one group to make it a control group makes it a powerful experimental design that covers the threats to internal validity. The posttest-only control-group design can be extended by adding additional treatment groups as shown in Figure 12-6. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

33 The Posttest-Only Control-Group Design
The posttest-only control-group design is a copy of the pretest-posttest control-group design but without the pretests included and is a copy of the two added groups in the Solomon four-group design. Random assignment to groups equates the two groups and withholding the IV from one group to make it a control group makes it a powerful experimental design that covers the threats to internal validity. The posttest-only control-group design can be extended by adding additional treatment groups as shown in Figure 12-6. Finally, we could create a factorial design from the posttest-only control group design by combining two of these designs simultaneously so that we ended up with a block diagram similar to those from Chapter 12. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

34 Conclusion How important is internal validity?
The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

35 Conclusion How important is internal validity?
It is the most important property of any experiment. If you do not concern yourself with the internal validity of your experiment, you are wasting your time. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

36 Single-Case Experimental Designs
An experiment that consists of one participant (also known as N = 1 designs). Includes controls just as in a typical experiment. Precautions are taken to insure internal validity. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

37 Single-Case Experimental Designs
Case-Study Approach An observational technique in which we compile a record of observations about a single participant. Often used in clinical settings. A case-study is merely a descriptive or observational approach. The researcher does not manipulate or control variables, but simply records observations. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

38 History of Single-Case Experimental Designs
In the 1860s Gustav Fechner explored sensory processes on an in-depth basis with a series of individuals. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

39 History of Single-Case Experimental Designs
In the 1860s Gustav Fechner explored sensory processes on an in-depth basis with a series of individuals. Wilhelm Wundt (founder of the first psychology laboratory) conducted his pioneering work on introspection with highly trained individual participants. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

40 History of Single-Case Experimental Designs
In the 1860s Gustav Fechner explored sensory processes on an in-depth basis with a series of individuals. Wilhelm Wundt (founder of the first psychology laboratory) conducted his pioneering work on introspection with highly trained individual participants. Hermann Ebbinghaus pioneered research in verbal learning and memory using himself as subject in a single-case design. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

41 History of Single-Case Experimental Designs
In the 1860s Gustav Fechner explored sensory processes on an in-depth basis with a series of individuals. Wilhelm Wundt (founder of the first psychology laboratory) conducted his pioneering work on introspection with highly trained individual participants. Hermann Ebbinghaus pioneered research in verbal learning and memory using himself as subject in a single-case design. Single-case designs declined in popularity as statistical tests for group experiments were developed in the 1920s. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

42 Uses of Single-Case Experimental Designs
There are still researchers who use single-case designs. Founded by B.F. Skinner, the experimental analysis of behavior approach continues to employ this technique. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

43 Uses of Single-Case Experimental Designs
Skinner (1966) summarized his philosophy in this manner: “Instead of studying a thousand rats for one hour each, or a hundred rats for ten hours each, the investigator is likely to study one rat for a thousand hours” (p. 21). The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

44 Uses of Single-Case Experimental Designs
Skinner (1966) summarized his philosophy in this manner: “Instead of studying a thousand rats for one hour each, or a hundred rats for ten hours each, the investigator is likely to study one rat for a thousand hours” (p. 21). The Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior was formed and began publishing its own journals Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior (in 1958) Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis (in 1968) The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

45 Uses of Single-Case Experimental Designs
Why use a single-case design in the first place? The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

46 Uses of Single-Case Experimental Designs
Why use a single-case design in the first place? Dukes (1965) provided a number of convincing arguments for and situations that require single-case designs. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

47 Uses of Single-Case Experimental Designs
Dukes (1965) arguments: The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

48 Uses of Single-Case Experimental Designs
Dukes (1965) arguments: A sample of one is all you can manage if that sample exhausts the population. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

49 Uses of Single-Case Experimental Designs
Dukes (1965) arguments: A sample of one is all you can manage if that sample exhausts the population. If you can assume perfect generalizability, then a sample of one is appropriate. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

50 Uses of Single-Case Experimental Designs
Dukes (1965) arguments: A sample of one is all you can manage if that sample exhausts the population. If you can assume perfect generalizability, then a sample of one is appropriate. A single-case design would be most appropriate when a single negative instance would refute a theory or an assumed universal relation. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

51 Uses of Single-Case Experimental Designs
Dukes (1965) arguments: A sample of one is all you can manage if that sample exhausts the population. If you can assume perfect generalizability, then a sample of one is appropriate. A single-case design would be most appropriate when a single negative instance would refute a theory or an assumed universal relation. You may simply have limitations on your opportunity to observe a particular behavior (e.g., H. M. ). The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

52 Uses of Single-Case Experimental Designs
Dukes (1965) arguments: A sample of one is all you can manage if that sample exhausts the population. If you can assume perfect generalizability, then a sample of one is appropriate. A single-case design would be most appropriate when a single negative instance would refute a theory or an assumed universal relation. You may simply have limitations on your opportunity to observe a particular behavior (e.g., H. M. ). When research is extremely time consuming and expensive, requires extensive training, or has difficulties with control, an investigator may choose to study only one participant (e.g., ape language studies). The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

53 General Procedures of Single-Case Experimental Designs
Hersen (1982) listed three procedures that are characteristic of single-case designs: The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

54 General Procedures of Single-Case Experimental Designs
Hersen (1982) listed three procedures that are characteristic of single-case designs: Repeated measures Baseline measurement Changing one variable at a time The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

55 General Procedures of Single-Case Experimental Designs
Repeated Measures When you are dealing with only one participant, it is important to make sure the behavior you are measuring is consistent. Therefore, you would repeatedly measure the participant’s behavior. Hersen and Barlow (1976) noted that the procedures for measurement “must be clearly specified, observable, public, and replicable in all respects (p. 71). Repeated measurements “must be done under exacting and totally standardized conditions with respect to measurement devices used, personnel involved, time or times of day measurements are recorded, instructions to the subject, and the specific environmental conditions” (p. 71). The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

56 General Procedures of Single-Case Experimental Designs
Baseline Measurement A measurement of behavior that is made under normal conditions (e.g., no IV is present); a control condition. Baseline measurement serves as the control condition against which to compare the behavior as affected by the IV. Barlow and Hersen (1973) recommend that you collect at least three observations during the baseline period in order to establish a trend in the data. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

57 General Procedures of Single-Case Experimental Designs
Changing One Variable at a Time In a single-case design it is vital that as the experimenter, you change only one variable at a time when you move from one phase of the experiment to the next. If you allow variables to change simultaneously, then you have a confounded experiment and cannot tell which variable has caused the change in behavior that you observe. If you record your baseline measurement, change several aspects of the participant’s environment, and then observe the behavior again, you have no way of knowing which changed aspect affected the behavior. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

58 Statistics and Single-Case Experimental Designs
Traditionally, researchers have not computed statistical analyses of results from single-case designs. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

59 Statistics and Single-Case Experimental Designs
Traditionally, researchers have not computed statistical analyses of results from single-case designs. The development of statistical tests for single-case designs has lagged behind multiple-case analyses. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

60 Statistics and Single-Case Experimental Designs
Traditionally, researchers have not computed statistical analyses of results from single-case designs. The development of statistical tests for single-case designs has lagged behind multiple-case analyses. There is also controversy about whether statistical analyses of single-case designs are even appropriate (Kazdin, 1976). The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

61 Statistics and Single-Case Experimental Designs
The Case Against Statistical Analysis The tradition has been to visually inspect (“eyeball”) the data to determine whether or not change has taken place. Statistical significance is not always the same as clinical significance. Even though statistical tests can help find effects that visual inspection cannot, such subtle effects may not be replicable (Kazdin, 1976). The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

62 Statistics and Single-Case Experimental Designs
The case for statistical analysis The argument for using statistical analyses of single-case designs revolves primarily around increased accuracy of conclusions. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

63 Statistics and Single-Case Experimental Designs
The Case for Statistical Analysis The argument for using statistical analyses of single-case designs revolves primarily around increased accuracy of conclusions. Jones, Vaught, and Weinrott (1977) found that conclusions drawn from visually inspected data were sometimes correct and sometimes incorrect. Both Type I and Type II errors occurred. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

64 Statistics and Single-Case Experimental Designs
The Case for Statistical Analysis The argument for using statistical analyses of single-case designs revolves primarily around increased accuracy of conclusions. Jones, Vaught, and Weinrott (1977) found that conclusions drawn from visually inspected data were sometimes correct and sometimes incorrect. Kazdin (1976) pointed out that statistical analyses are particularly likely to uncover findings that do not show up in visual inspection when a stable baseline is not established, new areas of research are being investigated, or testing is done in the real world, which tends to increase extraneous variation. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

65 Representative Single-Case Experimental Designs
Researchers use standard notation for single-case designs that makes the information easier to present and conceptualize. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

66 Representative Single-Case Experimental Designs
Standard Notation: The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

67 Representative Single-Case Experimental Designs
Standard Notation: A Refers to the baseline measurement in a single-case design. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

68 Representative Single-Case Experimental Designs
Standard Notation: B Refers to the outcome (treatment) measurement in a single-case design. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

69 Representative Single-Case Experimental Designs
The A-B Design The A-B design is the simplest of the single-case designs. We make baseline measurements, apply a treatment, and then take a second set of measurements. We compare the B (treatment) measurement to the A (baseline) measurement in order to determine whether a change has occurred. In the A-B design, the participant’s A measurements serve as the control for the B measurements. The A-B design is poor for determining causality because of many of the threats to internal validity (e.g., history, maturation, and instrumentation). The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

70 Representative Single-Case Experimental Designs
The A-B-A Design In the A-B-A design, the treatment phase is followed by a return to the baseline condition. If a change in behavior during B is actually due to the experimental treatment, the change should disappear when B is removed and you return to the baseline condition. If, on the other hand, a change in B was due to some extraneous variable, the change will not disappear when B is removed. Thus, the A-B-A design allows a causal relation to be drawn. If you end your experiment on an A phase, this leaves the participant “hanging” without the treatment. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

71 Representative Single-Case Experimental Designs
The A-B-A-B Design The A-B-A-B design begins with a baseline period followed by treatment, baseline, and treatment periods consecutively. This design adds a final treatment period to the A-B-A design, thereby completing the experimental cycle with the participant in a treatment phase. Hersen and Barlow (1976) point out that this design gives two transitions (B to A and A to B) that can demonstrate the effect of the treatment variable. Thus, our ability to draw a cause-and-effect conclusion is further strengthened. The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

72 Representative Single-Case Experimental Designs
Design and the real world From the preceding sections it should be clear that the A-B-A-B design is the preferred design for single-case research. But researchers often use A – B design despite limitations May be impractical to reverse a treatment May be unethical to reverse a treatment May be impossible, undesirable, or unethical to reverse a treatment if learning takes place during the treatment The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

73 Quasi-Experimental Designs
Virtually identical to true experimental designs with the exception of random assignment An IV is manipulated and a DV measured Cannot make causal inferences as groups may not be equal Necessary when random assignment not possible The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

74 Quasi-Experimental Designs
Nonequivalent group design involves two or more groups that are randomly assigned A comparison group is compared to one or more treatment groups Important to select comparison group as similar as possible The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education

75 Quasi-Experimental Designs
Nonequivalent group design involves two or more groups that are randomly assigned A comparison group is compared to one or more treatment groups Important to select comparison group as similar as possible Interrupted time-series design Involves a single group of participants Includes repeated pretreatment measures, an applied treatment, and repeated posttreatment measures History main threat to internal validity The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education


Download ppt "Chapter Thirteen: Alternative Research Designs"

Similar presentations


Ads by Google