Download presentation
Presentation is loading. Please wait.
Published byAndrea Navrátilová Modified over 6 years ago
1
How early can we begin assessing g with Coloured progressive matrices?
Gregor Sočan Tina Kavčič Maja Zupančič Department of Psychology University of Ljubljana
2
Coloured progressive matrices (CPM)
Raven’s progressive matrices (RPM) and the g factor CPM: children from about 5th year Pre-analogical operations (no complex abstract rules) Coefficient alpha (SLO): 0.85 – 0.92 (age 7-13) 3×12 items, increasing difficulty, 2-by-2 matrices
3
Illustrations of “rules”
Pattern completion
4
Illustrations of “rules”
Identity (repetition)
5
Illustrations of “rules”
Mirroring
6
Illustrations of “rules”
Change of colour/ pattern / form Subtraction
7
Problem and method “How useful are CPM for children below the age of seven?” 431 children, tested at the age of three (N = 302), four (N = 285) and six (N = 318) years. Analyses performed: Classical (unidimensionality, reliability etc.) Rasch scaling
8
Unidimensionality Is the hypothesis of perfect unidimensionality tenable? Structural equation modelling (not used here). Age 3: 14% Age 4: 19% Age 6: 28% First factor clearly dominant at age 4 and 6. Age 3: pattern completion vs. change in orientation and filling. What % of common variance is attributable to the general factor? Minimum rank factor analysis.
9
Reliability (lower bounds)
Age 3 4 6 Alpha .41 .55 .75 Bias-corrected greatest lower bound .61 .68 .83
10
Rasch analysis Model fit:
Number of items (out of 36) with fit MSQ > 1.3 Age 3 4 6 Unweighted MSQ 5 7 Information weighted MSQ Reasonable stability of item parameters over time: r’s between .93 and .97.
11
Item & person distribution: age 3
CPM.2E12 | CPM.2B11 | CPM.2B8 | CPM.2A11 | CPM.2A12 CPM.2AB6 CPM.2AB8 CPM.2B5 | CPM.2AB9 CPM.2B6 CPM.2B9 . | CPM.2B10 CPM.2B7 | CPM.2A9 # | CPM.2A10 CPM.2AB7 CPM.2A_B CPM.2A_C # + CPM.2B3 . | CPM.2A7 CPM.2AB4 .# | .## | CPM.2AB5 CPM.2A_A ###### + CPM.2B2 CPM.2B4 .######### | CPM.2AB3 ########## | ######## + CPM.2A8 CPM.2AB2 .############ | .######## | CPM.2A6 ##### + .#### | # + | CPM.2A5 # | | CPM.2AB1 | CPM.2A4 . | | CPM.2B1 CPM.2A1 | CPM.2A2 | CPM.2A3 Item & person distribution: age 3 person mean Low ability High ability Difficult items Easy items
12
Item & person distribution: age 4
| CPM.3E12 | | CPM.3B11 CPM.3B8 | CPM.3B9 | CPM.3A11 | CPM.3A12 CPM.3AB8 CPM.3B10 . | . | CPM.3AB9 CPM.3B5 CPM.3AB6 CPM.3A_B CPM.3A_C CPM.3B7 .# | .### | CPM.3AB7 CPM.3B6 #### + CPM.3A10 CPM.3A7 CPM.3A_A ###### | CPM.3A9 .###### | CPM.3AB4 CPM.3B3 0 ############# + CPM.3AB5 CPM.3B4 ##### | CPM.3B2 ###### | CPM.3A8 ### + CPM.3AB2 CPM.3AB3 .## | # + . | CPM.3A6 CPM.3A5 CPM.3AB1 | CPM.3A1 CPM.3A4 | CPM.3A3 CPM.3B1 CPM.3A2 person mean Low ability High ability Difficult items Easy items
13
Item & person distribution: age 6
CPM.4E12 | . | CPM.4B11 | CPM.4B8 . | CPM.4A11 . | CPM.4B9 CPM.4B10 # | CPM.4A12 .## | CPM.4AB8 CPM.4A_C ## | #### + ##### | CPM.4AB9 .##### | CPM.4A_B CPM.4B7 .######## | CPM.4A_A .###### | CPM.4B5 CPM.4B6 ##### + CPM.4AB6 .###### | CPM.4A7 CPM.4A9 CPM.4AB7 .########## | CPM.4A10 CPM.4A8 .### | ### + CPM.4AB4 CPM.4AB5 .#### | .# | CPM.4B3 CPM.4B4 ### | | CPM.4B2 # + . | CPM.4AB2 CPM.4AB3 | CPM.4A6 | CPM.4A5 | CPM.4AB1 | CPM.4A1 CPM.4A2 CPM.4A3 CPM.4A4 CPM.4B1 Item & person distribution: age 6 Low ability High ability person mean Difficult items Easy items
14
Test information function
15
Some interesting issues in brief:
Time stability: r34 = .32 r36 = .33 r46 = .43 True-score correlations (upper bounds): r34 = .49 r36 = .46 r46 = .57 No (statistically or practically) significant sex differences, including DIF.
16
Conclusions Conventional measurement standards met at the age of 6.
In certain circumstances (gifted children, group-level analysis…): 4 years. The validity question requires further investigation. In general, CPM is a high-quality test; some items still too easy/difficult.
Similar presentations
© 2025 SlidePlayer.com. Inc.
All rights reserved.