Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

1 Design of Experiments. 2 DESIGN OF EXPERIMENTS Purposeful changes of the inputs (factors) to a process in order to observe corresponding changes in.

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "1 Design of Experiments. 2 DESIGN OF EXPERIMENTS Purposeful changes of the inputs (factors) to a process in order to observe corresponding changes in."— Presentation transcript:

1 1 Design of Experiments

2 2 DESIGN OF EXPERIMENTS Purposeful changes of the inputs (factors) to a process in order to observe corresponding changes in the output (response). Process InputsOutputs Douglas Montgomery, Design and Analysis of Experiments

3 3 Why use DOE ? A basis of action -- allows purposeful changes. An analytic study -- one in which action will be taken on a cause-and-effect system to improve performance of a product or process in the future. Follows the scientific approach to problem solving. Provides a way to measure natural variation. Permits the clear analysis of complex effects. Most efficient way to derive the required information at the least expenditure of resources. Moen, Nolan and Provost, Improving Quality Through Planned Experimentation

4 4 Interactions Varying factors together vs. one at a time. BUCKBUCK D O E - - + + George Box, Do Interactions Really Matter, Quality Engineering, 1990.

5 5 BUCKBUCK D O E - - + + Voila! George Box, Do Interactions Really Matter, Quality Engineering, 1990.

6 6 Experiment run at SKF -- largest producer of rolling bearing in the world. Looked at three factors: heat treatment, outer ring osculation and cage design. Results: choice of cage design did not matter (contrary to previously accepted folklore -- considerable savings) life of bearing increased five fold if osculation and heat treatment are increased together -- saved millions of dollars ! George Box, Do Interactions Really Matter, Quality Engineering, 1990. Industry Example

7 7 Bearings like this have been made for decades. Why did it take so long to discover this improvement ? One factor vs. interaction effects ! Osculation Cage Heat 128 16 1921 26 85 17 25 George Box, Do Interactions Really Matter, Quality Engineering, 1990.

8 8 10621 18 23 Osculation Heat The Power of Interactions ! George Box, Do Interactions Really Matter, Quality Engineering, 1990.

9 9 2 Design Example 2 Consider an investigation into the effect of the concentration of the reactant and the amount of catalyst on the reaction time of a chemical process. L H reactant (factor A) 15% 25% catalyst (factor B) 1 bag 2 bags Douglas Montgomery, Design and Analysis of Experiments

10 10 Design Matrix for 2 2 ABABTotalAverage --+ +-- -+- +++ Main effects Interaction

11 11 Factor A - B -28252780 SettingsA + B -363232100 A - B +18192360 A + B +31302990 IIIIIITotal Replicates Douglas Montgomery, Design and Analysis of Experiments

12 12 An effect is the difference in the average response at one level of the factor versus the other level of the factor. - + A 60 90 80 100 A effect = ( [90 + 100] - [60 + 80] ) / 2(3) = 8.33 Douglas Montgomery, Design and Analysis of Experiments

13 13 Use a matrix to find the effects of each factor, including the interaction effect between the two factors. ABABTotalAverage --+8026.7 +--10033.3 -+-6020 +++9030 Avg + 31.7 Avg - 23.3 Effect 8.4 Douglas Montgomery, Design and Analysis of Experiments

14 14 ABABTotalAverage --+8026.7 +--10033.3 -+-6020 +++9030 Avg + 31.7 25 28.3 Avg - 23.3 30 26.7 Effect 8.4 -5 1.7 Completing the matrix with the effect calculations: Douglas Montgomery, Design and Analysis of Experiments

15 15 -10 -5 0 5 10 B AB A Dot Diagram Douglas Montgomery, Design and Analysis of Experiments

16 16 35 30 25 20 - + A Response Plots 35 30 25 20 - + B Douglas Montgomery, Design and Analysis of Experiments

17 17 35 30 25 20 - + A B - B + B - B + A - A + 26.7 20 33.3 30 Interaction Response Plot Douglas Montgomery, Design and Analysis of Experiments

18 18 Normal Probability Plots Effects are the differences between two averages. As we know, the distribution of averages are approximately normal. NPP can be used to identify the effects that are different from noise. Soren Bisgaard, A Practical Introduction to Experimental Design

19 19 Construction of NPP Can be constructed with effects on horizontal and cumulative percentages on vertical -- but this requires normal probability paper. Can also be constructed using the inverse standard normal of the plotting point ( (i -.5) / n ). Look for effects that are different from plotted ‘vertical’ reference line. Soren Bisgaard, A Practical Introduction to Experimental Design

20 20 Steps in constructing NPP 1. Compute effects. 2. Order effects from smallest to largest. 3. Let i be the order number (1 to n). 4. Calculate probability plotting position of the ordered effect using the formula ( p = [i -.5]/n). 5. Using a standard normal table determine the Z value corresponding to each left tail probability of step 4. 6. Plot the effects on horizontal axis and Z on vertical. 7. Fit a line through the most points. 8. Those ‘off the line’ are significant effects. Soren Bisgaard, A Practical Introduction to Experimental Design

21 21 3 4 5 2

22 22 Plot reference line through the majority of points. Look for effects which are off this line. 6 7 8

23 23 Prediction Equation The ‘intercept’ in the equation is the overall average of all observations. The coefficients of the factors in the model are 1/2 the effect. Y = 27.5 + 8.33/2 A - 5/2 B + 1.7/2 AB Y = 27.5 + 4.165 A - 2.5 B + 0.85 AB or note: A and B will be values between -1 and +1.

24 24 Analysis of Variance Source of Sum ofDegrees ofMeanF VariationSquaresFreedomSquare A208.331208.3353.15 * B 75.001 75.3319.13 * AB 8.331 8.33 2.13 Error 31.348 3.92 Total323.0011 * = significant at 1% (see F table)

25 25 Calculating SS, df and MS for Effects and Interactions Source of Sum ofDegrees ofMeanF VariationSquaresFreedomSquare A208.331208.3353.15 * SS = Effect 2 x n = 8.33 2 x 3 where n = replicates always 1 for this type design SS / df Use this same process for A, B and AB

26 26 Source of Sum ofDegrees ofMeanF VariationSquaresFreedomSquare Total323.0011 This is found by adding up every squared observation and then subtracting what is called a correction factor (sum of all observations, square this amount, then divide by the number of observations). SST = 28 2 + 25 2 + 27 2 +... + 29 2 - (330 2 / 12) = 9398.0 - 9075.0 = 323.0 Total df = n - 1 = 12 - 1 = 11 Calculating total sum of squares and total degrees of freedom

27 27 Source of Sum ofDegrees ofMeanF VariationSquaresFreedomSquare Error 31.348 3.92 Found by subtraction: Total SS - SS A - SS B - SS AB = 323 - 208.33 - 75.0 - 8.33 = 31.34 Found by subtraction: = Total df - A df - B df - AB df = 11 - 1 - 1 - 1 = 8 SS / df Calculating error sum of squares, df and mean square

28 28 Source of Sum ofDegrees ofMeanF VariationSquaresFreedomSquare A208.3353.15 * B 75.3319.13 * AB 8.33 2.13 Error 3.92 Calculating F ratios F ratios: F = MS (A or B or AB) ------------------------- MS (error) 208.33 / 3.92 75.33 / 3.92 8.33 / 3.92 Compare to F table

29 29 Interpreting F ratios F table at num df = 1 and denom df = 8 F.25 1.54 F.10 3.46 F.05 5.32 F.025 7.57 F.01 11.26 F ratios confirm that factors A and B are significant at the 1% level. F ratio shows there is not a significant interaction.

30 30 Exercise You will conduct a 2 3 experiment with 2 replicates. Factors:LH A -- Tower35 B -- Front Stop02 C -- Back Stop57

31 31 Requirements: 1. Collect data -- total of 16 observations (random order). 2. Fill in matrix and compute effects. 3. Put averages on a cube plot. 4. Plot effects on dot plot and normal probability plot. 5. Create appropriate response plots for significant interactions and main effects. 6. Interpret results and make recommendations to management.

32 32 Design Matrix

33 33 Cube Plot

34 34 Response Plots - + - +

35 35 Z Effect Normal Probability Plot

36 36 ANOVA table Source of Sum ofDegrees ofMeanF VariationSquaresFreedomSquare A B C AB AC BC ABC Error Total note: for 2 3 the SS =effect 2 x 2n

37 37 Why use 2 k designs ? Easy to use and data analysis can be performed using graphical methods. Relatively few runs required. 2 k designs have been found to meet the majority of the experimental needs of those involved in the improvement of quality. 2 k designs are easy to use in sequential experimentation. Fractions of the 2 k (fractional factorials) can be used to further reduce the experiment size. Moen, Nolan and Provost, Improving Quality Through Planned Experimentation

38 38 A review of the concepts behind Analysis of Variance

39 39 Analysis of Variance ANOVA is used to compare the means of two or more populations. Procedure is based on the spread (variance) between sample averages of populations and spread within sample averages. Possibly the most widely used procedure across disciplines.

40 40 Example Consider a cereal manufacturer who wants to evaluate the impact on sales of four package designs. Ten stores are randomly assigned to one of the designs and sales data are collected for a given period. This type of design is called a Completely Randomized Design.

41 41 It’s called analysis of VARIANCE! Recall that variance is the “almost average” of the squared differences of a set of data around its mean. For this set of data then, we have: (12 - 18) 2 + (14 - 18) 2 +... + (21 - 18) 2 = 304 units of variation

42 42 Variation What can account for this variation? type of package design (SSB) treatment everything else (SSE) error The total variation can be expressed in this relationship: SST = SSB + SSE

43 43 Why didn’t we sell the same amount of each package type? Why not the same at each store for package 1? (or 2?, or 3?, or 4?) ----- thousands of extraneous factors! SSE = (12 - 15) 2 + (18 - 15) 2 package 1 + (14 - 13) 2 + (12 - 13) 2 + (13 - 13) 2 package 2 + (19 - 19) 2 + (17 - 19) 2 + (21 - 19) 2 package 3 + (24 - 27) 2 + (30 - 27) 2 package 4 = 46 total units of variation

44 44 What number best represents the long-term average for package 1? The package 1 average (just as package 2 average does for package 2 designs, and so forth. How do these package averages vary from the overall average? (15 - 18) 2 + (13 - 18) 2 + (19 - 18) 2 + (27 - 18) 2 = 116 But, we must weigh each one by the number of observations in that average -- that gives us... 2(9) + 3(25) + 3(1) + 2(81) = 258 (note: 304 = 258 + 46)

45 45 OK -- we’ve got some ‘sum of squares’ But this procedure is called ‘analysis of variance’ FACT: variance = sum of squares divided by appropriate df Lets organize our results so far in an ANOVA table: Sources of variationSum of squares dfVariance SSB258386 SSE4667.67 Total3049

46 46 degrees of freedom As the name implies, this is the number of things that are free to vary and still get the same result. For example, if I told you the average of five numbers is 7, you could pick any four numbers, and if I can pick the fifth I can ensure the average is 7. Generally speaking, the df will be one less than the number of things being compared. For example, SSB df = 4(package designs) - 1 = 3 SSE df = (2 - 1) + (3 - 1) + (3 - 1) + (2 - 1) = 6 Total df = 10 - 1 = 9

47 47 A ratio of variances We next form a ratio of variances = 86 / 7.67 = 11.2 We need a reference distribution to evaluate this -- we compare it to the Fisher distribution (F distribution) A Short Fisher Table df forDegree ofdf for Numerator DenominatorConfidence123 29518.5119.0019.16 9998.5099.0099.17 6955.995.144.76 9913.7510.929.78

48 48 Making a decision We are really carrying out a hypothesis test. Our Ho is that are package design means are equal. H o : Our Ha is that at least one mean is different than the rest. We can make two decisions with our data: 1. No difference in means. This is one of those less than 1 in a 100 times we would get a value this large. 2. The null is false -- we reject the null and accept H a.

49 49 Summary of ANOVA concept 1. Decompose the total sum of squares. 2. Convert sum of squares into variances. 3. Compute variance ratio and compare to F table.

50 50 Assumptions! 1. Populations being compared are normally distributed -- moderate departures OK -- “robust” in this regard. 2. Variances of the populations are equal -- can be tested -- if this assumption is not met there is “trouble in River City.” 3. Observations are statistically independent (use randomization).


Download ppt "1 Design of Experiments. 2 DESIGN OF EXPERIMENTS Purposeful changes of the inputs (factors) to a process in order to observe corresponding changes in."

Similar presentations


Ads by Google