Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Chap.10 Foraging 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所.

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Chap.10 Foraging 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所."— Presentation transcript:

1 Chap.10 Foraging 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

2 Ayo 2010 Ethology2 Foraging  Finding food and the search image  Optimal foraging theory  What to eat  Where to eat  Specific nutrient constraints  Risk-sensitive foraging  Foraging and group life  Natural selection, phylogeny, and seed caching  Learning and foraging

3 Ayo 2010 Ethology3 Foraging  Searching for and consuming food, foraging, is a critical part of every animal’s existence.  Many animals spend a good deal of their waking hours foraging (Fig, 10.1)  (A) a black bear  (B) a Richardson ’ s ground squirrel

4 Ayo 2010 Ethology4

5 5 Ant-fungus relationship  About 50 million years ago, ants began cultivating their own food by entering into a mutually beneficial relationship with certain species of fungi.  The ants promote the growth of the fungi, while also feasting on the vegetative shoots produced by their fungal partners.  Aside from humans, ants are one of the few groups on the planet that grow their own food.  Fig. 10.2, A worker of the leaf-cutter ant tending a fungus garden. The thick whitish- gray coating of the worker is the mutualistic bacterium that produces the antibiotics that suppress the growth of parasite in the fungus garden.

6 Ayo 2010 Ethology6

7 7 Ant-fungus relationship 1.All twenty species of the fungus- growing ants examined had Streptomyces bacteria associated with them 2.Ants actually transmit the bacteria across generation, with parents passing the bacteria on to offspring. 3.Only females posses the bacteria. 4.The bacteria found on fungus-growing ants produce antibiotics that wipe out only certain parasitic diseases.

8 Ayo 2010 Ethology8 Finding food and the search image  When animals encounter a prey type more and more, they form some sort of representation of that target- the prey- and that this representation or image becomes more and more detailed with experience, which in turn translate into the forager becoming more successful at finding that type of prey (Tinbergen, 1960)

9 Ayo 2010 Ethology9 Optimal foraging theory (OFT)  What to eat?  Where to eat?  How long should a forager stay in a certain food patch?  Specific nutrient constraints  Risk-sensitive foraging  How does variance in food supply affect a forager ’ s decision about what food types to eat?

10 Ayo 2010 Ethology10 What to eat?  Cheetah (the forager)  Fig. 10.3 foraging decision.  Here a female cheetah has killed a hare (the prey)  In making the decision whether to take hares rather than some other prey, the animal will compare the energy value, encounter rate, and handling time for each putative prey.

11 Ayo 2010 Ethology11

12 Ayo 2010 Ethology12 Optimal prey choice model  The model assumes: 1.Energy intake from prey can be measured in some standard currency 2.Foragers can ’ t simultaneously handle one prey item and search for another 3.Prey are recognized instantly and accurately 4.Prey are encountered sequentially 5.Natural selection favors foragers that maximize their rate of energy intake.

13 Ayo 2010 Ethology13 Great tit foraging  One classic early experiment using optimal foraging theory had mealworms of different sizes presented on a conveyor belt to great tits. (Fig. 10.4)  Fig. 10.5 optimal choice of diet  (A) great tits  (B) bluegill sunfish The fit between expected and observed foraging is quite good, although the fish tended to oversample medium and small Dophnia in the high density treatment.

14 Ayo 2010 Ethology14

15 Ayo 2010 Ethology15 Great tits

16 Ayo 2010 Ethology16 Bluegill sunfish The fit between expected and observed foraging is quite good, although the fish tended to oversample medium and small Dophnia in the high density treatment.

17 Ayo 2010 Ethology17 Patch choice For a bee, different flowers in a field of flowering plants might represent different patches

18 Ayo 2010 Ethology18 Where to eat  Marginal value theorem( 邊際價值定律 ) (Charnov, 1976) 1.A forager should stay in a patch until the marginal rate of food intake – that is, the rate of food intake associated with the next prey item in its patch – is equal to that of the average rate of food intake across all patches available. 2.The greater the time between patches, the longer a forager should stay in a patch. 3.For patches that are already of generally poor equality when the forage enters the patch, individuals should stay longer in such patches than if they were foraging in an environment full of more profitable patches.  Fig. 10.7 graphical solution to marginal value problem

19 Ayo 2010 Ethology19

20 Ayo 2010 Ethology20

21 Ayo 2010 Ethology21 Great tits: optimal time in patch and travel time (Fig. 10.8)  (A) an artificial tree that allowed him to control both patch quality and travel time.  (B) the solid line is the predicted optimal time in a patch plotted against the travel time, which was calculated based on the marginal value theorem, while the data points are the observed times the birds stayed in the patch plotted as a function of travel time between patches.  The results clearly demonstrate that the amount of time birds spent in a patch matched the optimal time predicted by the marginal value theorem.

22 Ayo 2010 Ethology22

23 Ayo 2010 Ethology23 Specific nutrient constraints  Fig. 10.9 Moose foraging on a salt budget.  Sodium is a particularly good candidate for a nutrient constraint study because vertebrates require large amounts of sodium, sodium is scarce, and besides water, sodium is the only nutrient for which a “specific hunger” has been documented in animals.  Moose need salt, and they acquire it from energy-poor plants. This takes time away from foraging on energy-rich terrestrial plants. (Fig. 10.10)

24 Ayo 2010 Ethology24

25 Ayo 2010 Ethology25

26 Ayo 2010 Ethology26 Risk-sensitive foraging  Risk, the term was first used in economics, where more variance implied a greater chance of loss (or gain).  Increased variance in prey availability increases.  Rick-sensitive optimal foraging models (Fig. 10.11)  2 patches, patch 1 that always yields eight pellets once the cover is removed  Patch 2 in which half the time there are no pellets and half the time there are sixteen pellets.  Both patches have the same mean (eight), but the variance is greater in patch 2.  If forager takes variance into account, it is foraging in a rick-sensitive manner.

27 Ayo 2010 Ethology27

28 Ayo 2010 Ethology28 Forager, 3 different hunger states  Forager 1 has a hunger stat, in which it values every new food item equality.  Risk insensitive (Fig. 10.12 (A))  Forager 2 is fairly satiated ( 相當飽足 ), and although every additional item it takes in has some value, each additional item is worth less and less.  Risk adverse (Fig. 10.13 (B))  Forager 3 is starving, and every additional item it eats is worth more and more (to a limit).  Risk prone (Fig. 10.13 (C) )

29 Ayo 2010 Ethology29 Fig. 10.12 (A) hunger : Risk insensitive Fig. 10.13 (B) fairly satiated ( 相當飽足 ) : Risk adverse Fig. 10.13 (C) starving : Risk prone

30 Ayo 2010 Ethology30 Rule of thumb  As with all the mathematical models we analyze, we are not suggesting that animals make the mental calculations that we just went through, but rather that natural selection favors any “rule- of-thumb behavior.  The favored rule-of-thumb might be “when starving, use patches of food that have high variances.”

31 Ayo 2010 Ethology31 Junco foraging behavior has been used to test numerous optimal foraging models. Fig. 10.14. utility functions and risk sensitivity (A) risk-prone juncos. The utility function for this junco indicates that each additional item the bird eats is worth more and more. (B) Risk adverse juncos. Each additional item a junco receives is worth less and less.

32 Ayo 2010 Ethology32 Risk adverseRisk prone

33 Ayo 2010 Ethology33 Foraging and Group Life  Foraging in a group  Increasing the foraging group size increases the amount of food each forager receives.  Foraging in bluegills (Fig. 10.15, 16, 17)  Disentangling( 解開 ) the effect of group size and cooperation on foraging success.  Wild dogs  Chimp (Tai chimp vs. Gombe chimp)

34 Ayo 2010 Ethology34 Foraging in bluegills  Fig. 10.16 group size and foraging success. In bluegill sunfish, the mean rate of prey captured increases with group size until group size reaches about four individuals.  Flushing effect, when bluegills forage in groups, they flush out more prey and attract other fish to the foraging site. (Fig. 10.17)  Meta-analysis on foraging success and group size in seven different species that hunt in groups. Overall, a strong positive relationship between foraging success and group size.

35 Ayo 2010 Ethology35 Bluegill sunfish forage for small aquatic insects in dense vegetation. The bluegills’ foraging patterns approximate those predicted by theory.

36 Ayo 2010 Ethology36

37 Ayo 2010 Ethology37

38 Ayo 2010 Ethology38 Disentangling the effect of group size and cooperation on foraging success  Individuals may cooperate with one another when hunting in groups. For example, wild dogs  Cooperative hunting in chimp populations, Tai chimps and Gombe chimps.  Tai chimps, cooperation hunting  Gombe chimps, no correlation between group size and hunting success.  The success rate for Gombe solo hunters was quite high compared with the individual success rate for Tai chimps.

39 Ayo 2010 Ethology39

40 Ayo 2010 Ethology40 Groups and public information  in public information models, individuals simply use the actions of others as a means of assessing the condition of the environment, and as such, public information allows group members to reduce environmental uncertainty.  Solitary foragers vs. foragers in a group.  Starlings were tested using an array of food placed into cups. (Fig. 10.19)

41 Ayo 2010 Ethology41 Public information in starlings  A given bird (B1) fed from such a feeder either alone or paired with a second bird (B2).  Prior to being paired with B1 partners, B2 birds had either been given the chance to sample a few cups in this feeder, or to sample all such cups.  Two results support the predictions of public information models.  When tested on completely empty feeding patches, B1 birds left such patches earlier when paired with any B2 bird than when foraging alone.  B1 birds left patches earliest of all when paired with B2 birds that had complete information about the patches.

42 Ayo 2010 Ethology42

43 Ayo 2010 Ethology43 Natural selection, phylogeny and seed caching  Hippocampal ( 海馬體的 ) size and caching ability  To be associated with food retrieval. (food storage)  Fig. 10.20 Foraging and brain size. The volume of the hippocampal region relative to body mass was positively correlated with the extent of food storing in six species of birds,  Phylogeny and caching ability A.Alpine cough B.Jackdaw C.Rook and crow combined D.Red-billed blue magpie E.Magpie F.European jay

44 Ayo 2010 Ethology44 A.Alpine cough B.Jackdaw C.Rook and crow combined D.Red-billed blue magpie E.Magpie F.European jay

45 Ayo 2010 Ethology45 Chickadees ( 山雀 ) from Colorado or Alaska  Bring them back to laboratory at the University of California at Davis.  The results:  The birds from Alaska (food-scarce population) cached a greater percentage of seeds than the birds from Colorado (food- rich population).  The Alaska birds found more of their cached seeds than did the Colorado birds, and their searches were more efficient in that they made fewer errors (Fig. 10.21)

46 Ayo 2010 Ethology46

47 Ayo 2010 Ethology47 Phylogeny and caching ability  Evolutionary history of caching behavior in the corvid family ( 鴨科 ).  Phylogeny of 46 species  Non-cachers  Moderate cachers  Specialized cachers  Result:  The ancestral state of caching in corvids is “ moderate caching ”.

48 Ayo 2010 Ethology48 Learning and foraging  Foraging, learning, and brain size in birds  Hypothesized a neurobiological link between forebrain size and learning abilities in animals.  Table 10.1 examples of foraging innovations in birds. Data on 322 foraging innovations, including those in this list.  Relative forebrain size correlated with foraging innovation. Larger forebrains were more likely to have high incidences of foraging innovation (Fig. 10.22)  Learning and planning for the future  Social learning and foraging

49 Ayo 2010 Ethology49

50 Ayo 2010 Ethology50

51 Ayo 2010 Ethology51

52 Ayo 2010 Ethology52

53 Ayo 2010 Ethology53 Planning for the future  If animals could plan for the future based on prior experience, as we humans clearly do, there would be huge fitness benefits associated with such an ability.  Two requirements  The behavior must be novel, so that we can be certain that we are not seeding the manifestation of some innate action  The behavior in question must not be tied to the current motivational state of the animal, but rather to the anticipated motivational state at some point in the future.  案例: Western scrub jays modify their foraging behavior in an attempt to plan for the future (Fig. 10.24)

54 Ayo 2010 Ethology54

55 Ayo 2010 Ethology55 Western scrub jays and planning for the future  On alternate morning over the course of six days, birds were exposed to one of two compartments- one compartment contained food in the form of ground-up pine nuts, and the other compartment contained no food.  On the evening before each test, the birds were not fed any food, and they were therefore hungry during their exposure to test compartments.  After the six days of exposure to the two compartments, the birds were denied access to any food for two hours before dark, and then they were unexpectedly provided with a bowl of whole pine nuts – that is, food that could be cached.  Jays cached more nuts in the compartment in which they had consistently received no food in the past.

56 Ayo 2010 Ethology56 Social learning and foraging in pigeons  Fig. 10.25 urban foragers. Pigeons are scavengers, coming across novel food items all the time. Such a species is ideal for study foraging and cultural transmission.  Fig. 10.26 Pigeons in this experiment need to learn to pierce the red half of paper covering a box of seed. The graph shows average latency to eating for four groups: NM (no model) group, Bl (blind imitation) group, LE (local enhancement) group, and OL (observational learning) group.  Pigeons in NM and Bl treatments never learned to feed in the experimental apparatus. The quickest learning occurred in the OL treatment.

57 Ayo 2010 Ethology57

58 Ayo 2010 Ethology58

59 Ayo 2010 Ethology59 Producers and scroungers  Producers find and procure food  Scroungers make their living parasitizing the food that producers have uncovered.  Fig. 10.27 producers and scroungers  when a group member finally opens a tube with food in it, the food spills on the floor and is accessible to all. Out of sixteen pigeons, only two learned to open tubes, while fourteen acted as scroungers.

60 Ayo 2010 Ethology60

61 Ayo 2010 Ethology61 Scrounging prevents social learning  Fig. 10.28 Scrounging prevents social learning  (A) one group of birds saw a model bird peck at a stick in a rubber stopper at the end of the tube. This provided food to the model. These birds learned to peck,  (B) scroungers fared much worse than non-scroungers when faced with the task of pecking at the stick.

62 Ayo 2010 Ethology62

63 Ayo 2010 Ethology63

64 Ayo NUTN website: http://myweb.nutn.edu.tw/~hycheng/ 問題與討論


Download ppt "Chap.10 Foraging 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所."

Similar presentations


Ads by Google