Download presentation
Presentation is loading. Please wait.
Published byRodney Rodgers Modified over 9 years ago
1
Multi-tasking on the Information Super Highway: Why Using a Cell Phone Can Make You Drive Like You’re Drunk David Strayer Department of Psychology RMPA: April 13, 2007
2
Research Questions Does conversing on a cell phone interfere with driving? What are the sources of the interference? Peripheral interference (dialing, holding the phone) Attentional interference (cell phone conversation) How significant is the interference?
3
Observational Study (Residential 4-way Intersections) Odds ratio for failing to stop: 0.27 for drivers not using a cell phone 2.93 for drivers using a cell phone Traffic Violation No Traffic Violation On Cell 8228110 Not on Cell 35212861838 43413141748
4
Epidemiological Studies (Case Crossover Design) Redelmeier & Tibshirani (1997) NEJM 699 driver involved in a non-injury automobile accident 4-fold increase in risk of accident when using cell phone McEvoy et al., (2005) BMJ 456 drivers requiring hospital attendance after automobile accident 4-fold increased likelihood of crashing when using a cell phone
5
Driving Simulator Studies (Car Following Paradigm) Drivers conversing on a cell phone were 5 times more likely to be involved in a traffic accident Traffic Accident No Accident Single-Task 2119121 Dual-Task 10111121 12230242
6
High-Fidelity Driving Simulator
7
Simulator-Based Studies Car-following paradigm Follow periodically braking pace car Required timely and appropriate reactions Hands-free cell phone (positioned in advance) Conditions Single vs. dual-task Low vs. moderate density * Measures Reaction time Following distance Rear-end collisions Low Mod. SingleDual
8
Reaction Time
9
Following Distance
10
Rear-end Collisions
11
Summary (Experiment 1) Cell-phone driver’s Slower reaction times Drivers compensate by increasing following distance Increase in rear-end accidents Cell-phone interference Even when manual contributions are eliminated Naturalistic conversations
12
Why Do Cell Phones Cause Interference? From earlier studies, no interference from: Radio broadcasts (audio input) Books on tape & recorded conversations (audio/verbal input) Simple shadowing (audio/verbal input, verbal output) Implies active engagement in conversation necessary Impairments from both hand-held and hands-free units Implies central / cognitive locus Inattention-blindness (James, Neisser, Simons)
13
Why Do Cell Phones Cause Interference? From earlier studies, no interference from: Radio broadcasts (audio input) Books on tape & recorded conversations (audio/verbal input) Simple shadowing (audio/verbal input, verbal output) Implies active engagement in conversation necessary Impairments from both hand-held and hands-free units Implies central / cognitive locus Inattention-blindness (James, Neisser, Simons)
14
Why Do Cell Phones Cause Interference? From earlier studies, no interference from: Radio broadcasts (audio input) Books on tape & recorded conversations (audio/verbal input) Simple shadowing (audio/verbal input, verbal output) Implies active engagement in conversation necessary Impairments from both hand-held and hands-free units Implies central / cognitive locus Inattention-blindness (James, Neisser, Simons)
15
Experiment 2: Inattention-Blindness Test for evidence of cell-phone induced inattention blindness High-fidelity driving simulator Hands-free cell phone Naturalistic conversation with confederate Eye tracker Two phases to the study: Phase 1: Single & dual-task driving Phase 2: Recognition memory tests for objects encountered while driving
16
Recognition Memory
17
Recognition Memory Given Fixation
18
Experiment 2a: Summary 50% drop in recognition memory from single to dual-task, consistent with inattention blindness interpretation What about items more relevant to safe driving? Do drivers divert attention from processing items of low task relevance (e.g., billboards), but protect high task relevance items (e.g., pedestrians)?
19
Experiment 2b: Effects of Traffic Relevance Phase I: Single & dual-task driving Interstate driving (with traffic) Hands-free cell phone, naturalistic conversations Unique items placed in single & dual-task scenarios Phase II: Surprise 2AFC recognition memory test Single-task items (driving only) Dual-task items (driving & phone) Control items (not seen while driving)
20
Driving Safety Relevance Ratings
21
2AFC Recognition Memory Given Fixation (Corrected for Guessing) Dual-task interference not modulated by driving safety relevance
22
Experiment 2: Summary Cell phone conversations create inattention blindness for traffic related events/scenes Cell phone drivers look but fail to see up to half of the information in the driving environment No evidence that cell phone drivers protect more traffic relevant information
23
Experiment 3: Encoding or Retrieval Deficits? Encoding deficits Reduced attention to perceptual inputs Clear implications for traffic safety Retrieval deficits Failure to retrieve prior episodes Less clear implications for traffic safety Event-related brain potentials recorded to traffic brake lights Single-task Dual-task
24
Traffic-related Brain Activity
25
Experiment 3: Summary Brain waves suppressed by cell phone conversations Cell phone conversations impair encoding of information necessary for the safe operation of a motor vehicle
27
Cell Phone vs. Passenger Conversations Conditions Single task / dual task Conversing on cell phone Conversing with passenger Design Task (2) x Condition (2) Single task Cell Passenger
28
Lane Keeping Errors
29
Successful Navigation
30
Traffic References
31
Summary (Experiment 4) Cell-phone conversations More navigation errors Fewer references to traffic Passenger conversations Collaborative problem solving Shared situation awareness Passenger actively supports the driver
32
Experiment 5: How Significant is the Interference? Cell-phone vs. drunk-driver Redelmeier and Tibshirani (1997) reported epidemiological evidence suggesting that “the relative risk [of being in a traffic accident while using a cell-phone] is similar to the hazard associated with driving with a blood alcohol level at the legal limit” (p. 465).
33
Cell-phone Driver vs. Drunk Driver Car-following paradigm Follow periodically braking pace car Required timely and appropriate reactions Conditions Single-task driving Cell-phone driving * Intoxicated driving (BAC= 0.08 wt/vol) * Hands-free = Hand-held
34
Reaction Time
35
Following Distance
36
Rear-end Collisions
37
Summary (Experiment 5) Compared to drunk driver, cell-phone driver’s reactions Slower reaction times Longer to recover lost speed following braking Drivers compensate by increasing following distance Increase in rear-end accidents When controlling for time on task and driving conditions, cell-phone drivers’ performance is worse than that of the drunk driver
38
You Cannot Practice Away The Interference Cooper & Strayer et al., (2007) HFES Accident Frequency for City and Highway Driving Day 1 Training Day 4 Training Day 4 Transfer Single Task 1661032 Dual Task 25122663 41183695
39
Teen Drivers More at Risk Chisholm et al., (2006) HFES Accident Percentage – pedestrian and pullout vehicle events Single-TaskDual-Task Novice (< 6 months) 3.3%6.4%4.85% Experienced (10 years) 0.0%2.5%1.25% 1.65%4.45%
40
Research Questions Does conversing on a cell phone interfere with driving? Yes What are the sources of the interference? Peripheral interference (dialing, text messaging) Attentional interference (inattention blindness) How significant is the interference? Worse than listening to radio/books on tape Worse than in-vehicle conversations More impairing than driving while intoxicated at legal limit
Similar presentations
© 2025 SlidePlayer.com. Inc.
All rights reserved.