Download presentation
Presentation is loading. Please wait.
Published byDelilah Cameron Modified over 9 years ago
1
Cultural Psychology Daniel S. Messinger, Ph.D.
2
Cultural Psychology All social and emotional development occurs in a cultural context Culture involves shared beliefs and practices which unite communities and differentiate them from other communities What may appear to be a universal feature of development, is often one of myriad, cultural solutions to a problem
3
Examples What to do when baby cries Where should baby sleep Who should play with baby Who should take care of baby What about rambunctious toddlers
4
Questions What is cultural psychology (give examples)? Is psychology we’ve been studying cultural psychology? How are toddlers’ desires for objects handled differently in Salt Lake City and San Pedro? Do toddlers or siblings end up with object in each community and what do mothers believe about this? What are differences between American and Japanese toddlers in toddler task and do they reflect differences in autonomy and interdependence – have reference to videotapes examples What types of attributions characterize traditional Japanese child-rearing? What is the developmental discontinuity in Japanese development? Child care How is the quantity and quality of child care associated with peer competence?
5
Smile imitation differs by 3 months Wörmann, V., Holodynski, M., Kärtner, J., & Keller, H. (2012). A cross-cultural comparison of the development of the social smile: A longitudinal study of maternal and infant imitation in 6- and 12- week-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(3), 335-347. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.03.002
6
Current trend of human development Emphasizing the meaning of both individual and cultural factors of socialization Previously, individual autonomy, defined as independence from others: a requisite of healthy human development Celimli
7
Autonomy vs. Interdependence? Value system, rules, and the structure of the family unit have been formed through the societal demands which show variances across time and cultures. A model of family change (Kağıtçıbaşı, 1996a, 1996b) - analyzes the link between the self, family, and society in order to explain cultural differences Celimli
8
Family Interaction Patterns (Kagitcibasi, 1996, 2005) Pattern of total interdependence: The child is the economic value Independence of the child is not valued or evaluated Obedience is fundamental to childrearing Pattern of dependence: The child: a source of economic costs Independence of the child: highly valued Autonomy is the basic childrearing orientation The pattern of psychological interdependence: The child: no longer the economic value Psychological interdependence of the child: valued Closeness and relatedness (not separateness) is the ultimate goal in childrearing practices Celimli
9
Physical contact & tactile behaviors Hispanic mothers report touching more frequently, being more affectionate with their infants and having more skin-to-skin contact. Observations: no overall differences in mother- infant touch, but the Hispanic mothers showed more close touch and more close and affectionate touch compared to Anglo mothers, who showed more distal touch. Infants were 9 months old Franco F, Fogel A, Messinger DS, Frazier CA. Cultural Differences in Physical Contact Between Hispanic and Anglo Mother-Infant Dyads Living in the United States. Early Development and Parenting 1996;5(3):119 - 127.
10
What about Miami? Does parental touch differ between Hispanics and non-Hispanics? What functions might differences serve? Assortative mating
11
Tronick, Morelli and Ivey (1992) Continuous Care and Contact model Primary relationship with one individual that widens only to one or two more people. Evidence of this in US Infants, and cross-culturally from the !Kung (Konner 1972) Mattson
12
Efe People Ituri forest of Zaire Small communities of a few families Leaf huts face a communal space High mortality rates, low fertility High work load, not strong delineations of labor most gathering is communal Multiple caregivers, infants engaged with others half the time, 5+ every hour (Tronick et al. 1987) Methodology issues Naturalistic, one community at both points Data gathered at different times Better data gathering methods Researchers living in these camps Some variation in when the observations were made Mattson
13
Multiple, simultaneous relationships ‘The Efe infant experiences a pattern of simultaneous and multiple relationships. Not a pattern initially focused on one person that gradually progresses to other relationships. This simultaneous pattern is influenced by physical and social ecological factors and cultural practices. This pattern of social experience leads to a sense of self that incorporates other people Assumptions that early relationships are hierarchical and sequential in nature, require reevaluation.’ Tronick, E. Z., Morelli, G. A., & Ivey, P. K. (1992). The Efe forager infant and toddler's pattern of social relationships: Multiple and simultaneous. Developmental Psychology, 28(4), 568-577. The Efe forager infant and toddler's pattern of social relationships: Multiple and simultaneous.
14
Caretaker-Child Strategic model Motivation and goals, opportunities, constraints between infant and caregiver BUT... Social Factors - Group Composition, values customs -> child rearing patterns Ecological Factors - climate, food supply, environmental risks -> effort, protection and care VS. Mattson
15
Method and Results Quasi-cross sectional (Five Levels, N = 40, n ~= 8, gender about the same) Four groups- Mother, Father, Adults, Children Compare to average person for Adults and Children, rather than the group as a whole for timing Solitary activity increases with age and Social contact decrease with age Overall less time spent with Mothers across the first three years, no significant difference for Fathers At first time point, Mothers more than father or adults, Children more than Fathers At last time point, More time with Children than any other, More contact with Mother than Father. More time spent with average child over the first three years At 5 months spent more time with mother than others, at 3 years Peers and mothers are similar, more children than adults. Mattson
16
Conclusions and Further Questions Tronick suggests Society requires managing many relationships which are essential to be successful Close distances to work areas mean easy to return and console/tend to infant, in contrast with !Kung Mortality leads to many parent-less children and childless adults Care-giving style works to buffer from health risks Many people work as many "secure" bases Therefore this pattern suggest a more multi-layered process were each relationship may have an independent and meaningful impact. Is this exclusionary of CCC model? Mattson
17
Time alone? Time with whom?
19
Privileged Treatment of Toddlers Cultural Aspects of Individual Choice and Responsibility Christine E. Mosier Barbara Rogoff Developmental Psychology 2003, 39, 1047-1060
20
Privileged Treatment of Toddlers: Cultural Aspects of Individual Choice and Responsibility. Christine Mosier and Barbara Rogoff Collectivism v. Individualism? Treatment of toddlers: Special treatment or same rules for sharing? Age of understanding Emphasis on individuality and choice promotes cooperation that is voluntary as opposed to guided by parental control The study: 16 Mayan families from San Pedro, Guatemala 16 middle class families from Salt Lake City, Utah Interactions between toddlers (14 to 20 mo) and siblings (3 to 5 yrs) Interview with mother about child-rearing, social behavior, etc. Given 9 objects to toddlers and siblings to manipulate, with mother’s help Nayfeld
21
Access to Desired Objects How 3- to 5-year-old siblings and mothers handled access to objects desired by the siblings and toddlers, in Mayan families of San Pedro, Guatemala, and middle-class families in Salt Lake City, Utah. We observed whether toddlers (14–20 months) were accorded privileged access to objects that their siblings also desired or whether toddlers and slightly older siblings were held to similar expectations.
22
Guatemalan Mayan mothers “almost never overruled their toddlers' objections to or insistence on an activity—they attempted to persuade but did not force the child to cooperate toddlers were not compelled to stop hitting others. [Toddler] hitting was not regarded as motivated by an intent to harm because they were expected to be too young to understand the consequences of their acts for other people.” Mosier & Rogoff, 2003
24
Interview your partner 1. What do you do if the children fight over a toy? What if it belongs to the toddler? To the sibling? 2. When do you think children begin to understand the consequences of their acts, for example, regarding places not to go and things not to touch? 3. If the toddler were to destroy something, how would you handle it? Would she/he be punished? How? Is it possible for the toddler to destroy things on purpose at this age? If so, when did the toddler begin to understand? Is it possible for the sibling to destroy things on purpose? When did the sibling begin to understand? 4. Does the toddler understand that hitting or pulling hair hurts? If so, at what age did the toddler begin to understand? When did the sibling begin to understand that what he/she does might hurt someone?
25
Proportions of Events Regarding Access to an Object Nayfeld Event Salt Lake CitySan Pedro Toddlers eventually gained access to the object.59 (.20).87 (.09) Mothers endorsed toddler’s privileged position.43 (.24).63 (.22) Mothers endorsed toddler’s nonprivileged position.25 (.13).04 (.05) Siblings endorsed toddler’s privileged position.45 (.19).80 (.09) Siblings endorsed toddler’s nonprivileged position.54 (.21).19 (.09)
26
Maternal education (acculturation) San Pedro mothers’ schooling related negatively to their privileged endorsements (r.50, p <.05) and uninvolvement (r.57, p <.05) and related positively to their nonprivileged endorsements (r.56, p <.05).
27
Affects siblings Salt Lake City: The older brother (3 years 0 months) was playing with the pencil box and lid. Sam (15 months) wanted the lid and grabbed for it. A tug-of-war ensued until both mother and father separated the two boys. The older brother ended up with the complete box and Sam ended up playing with the embroidery hoop. San Pedro: The older brother (3 years 9 months) put his hand on the knob of the jar lid. Lidia (15 months) reached out and gently pushed his hand off. The brother removed his hand. Reverse dominance hierarchies?
28
A hefty 15-month-old… walked around bonking his brothers and sisters, his mother, and his aunt with the stick puppet that I had brought along. The adults and older children just tried to protect themselves and the little children near them, they did not try to stop him. When I asked local people what this toddler had been doing, they commented, “He was amusing people; he was having a good time.” Was he trying to hurt anybody? “Oh no. He couldn't have been trying to hurt anybody; he's just a baby. He wasn't being aggressive, he's too young; he doesn't understand. Babies don't [misbehave] on purpose.” (p. 165)
29
Other transitions In the Marquesas (Polynesia), the goal is likewise to coordinate personal goals with group goals. Marquesan toddlers go through a transition at 18–24 months resembling that of the San Pedro toddlers, from having every demand met to being expected to cooperate:. The ideal [Marquesan] situation is one in which people have similar or complementary goals and willingly collaborate in a mutually beneficial activity without anyone dominating anyone else.
30
Kids’ Attention to Interactions Directed to Others Guatemalan Mayan & European American Patterns of Learning Indigenous: intent community participation Attending to ongoing events -- pitch in when ready Middle-class: lessons not in context of productive activity Differences Indigenous toddlers/parents showed “keen simultaneous attention” EA toddlers/parents “quickly alternated their attention” Schooling often connected with parental attention management Method 3 groups: Traditional Mayan, Kaxlaan Mayan, European-American Session 1: observed sibling learning how to make a toy Coded for attention to sibling’s lesson, disruption/attention seeking, and attempts to collaborate Session 2: Ten days later, put together sibling’s toy Coded for how much help the child needed Correa-Chavez & Rogoff, 2009 Kolnik
31
Results Session 1: Attending Mayan children engaged in more sustained attention than EA children EA children engaged in more brief glances and not attending EA children disrupted more than Mayan children Session 2: Frog toy learning Effect of background on need for help Traditional Mayan < Kaxlaan Mayan < EA Effect of sustained attention on need for help Children with more sustained attention needed less help Effect of sustained attention, CONTROLLING for background Sustained attention significant, background not significant Kolnik No differen ces on attempt s to collabor ate
32
Silva, Correa-Chavez, and Rogoff Cultural variation in children’s attentiveness Indigenous vs. westernized learning styles Pueblo basic school vs. Mexican high schooling Maternal education level and cultural traditions Toy construction paradigm Bustamante
33
Silva, Correa-Chavez, and Rogoff Bustamante
34
Silva, Correa-Chavez, and Rogoff Bustamante
35
Child and Mother Play in Three U.S Cultural Groups (Cote & Bornstein, 2009) Mother-child dyads from three cultural backgrounds living in the U.S. European American mother-child dyads South American Latino mother-child dyads Japanese mother-child dyads Celimli
36
Cultural Differences in Child’s Play (Cote & Bornstein, 2009) European American children – engage more exploratory or object-related play Argentina and Japan children – engage in more symbolic, person-directed play Why? Celimli
37
Interdependence vs. autonomy? American children are socialized to be relatively autonomous, while Japanese children are socialized to work interdependently in groups (Benedict, 1974; Conroy et ah, 1980). Japanese mothers more frequently focus their infants' attention within the mother-infant dyad, while American infants spend more time engaged with toys and vocalize or initiate vocalizations more frequently (Bornstein et al., 1985-1986; Caudiil and Weinstein, 1969; Shand and Kosawa, 1985)
38
Japan “being voluntarily cooperative, sunao, is encouraged: A child who is sunao has not yielded his or her personal autonomy for the sake of cooperation; cooperation does not suggest giving up the self as it may in the West; it implies that working with others is the appropriate way of expressing and enhancing the self. … How one achieves a sunao child … seems to be never go against the child.”
39
Child and Mother Play in Three U.S Cultural Groups (Cote & Bornstein, 2009) Participants: 20-month-olds and their mothers: South American Latino immigrants (37), Japanese immigrants (36), and European Americans (40). Procedure: The child play alone (10 min); The child-mother play (10 min) with an identical toy set
40
Findings (Cote & Bornstein, 2009) Boys – more exploratory and less symbolic play than girls for all groups Both Latino and Japanese children – more exploratory and less symbolic play when they alone Japanese children – more exploratory and less symbolic play when they alone than play was initiated by mother
41
Findings (Cote & Bornstein, 2009) Children’s play tends to be more sophisticated when their mothers encourage them than when children initiate play or play alone No cultural differences here
42
Turtle task. 24- to 31-month-olds Japanese mothers more frequently assisted their toddlers in fitting a shape before the toddlers had tried to fit the shape on their own (interdependence); American toddlers did not attempt to fit more shapes on their own (autonomy); More American toddlers left the task than did Japanese toddlers (autonomy).
43
Figure
44
Video: Turtle task
45
Apparent developmental discontinuity While the desires of Japanese infants are indulged, school-age children are expected to regulate their desires to conform to the demands of working in a group (Hendry, 1986). A sharp contrast is thought to exist between the infant-centered relationship with the mother in the home and the expectation that 3-year-olds, upon entrance to nursery school, will learn to conform to shudan seikatsu, 'life in a group' (Peak, 1989)
46
Japanese discontinuity In traditional practices that may seem indulgent to Western eyes, Japanese mothers do not go against the young child's will but use other ways to foster self-motivated cooperation Traditional Japanese belief holds that it is not appropriate to control young children from the outside, because use of controlling behavior (such as anger or impatience) leads children after age 10 or 11 to resent and disobey authority rather than to cooperate with others
47
Differential attributions Children are never willfully bad What are the results of such a belief?
48
How to study Should we take a (likely Western) construct and apply it across cultures E.g.:Italian mothers were more sensitive and optimally structuring, and Italian children were more responsive and involving, than Argentine and U.S. dyads.’ (Bornstein et al. 2008) Or should we adopt constructs from the cultures we are studying? E.g. Rogoff What about subcultures here in Miami?
Similar presentations
© 2025 SlidePlayer.com. Inc.
All rights reserved.