Download presentation
Presentation is loading. Please wait.
1
LINEAR REGRESSIONS: Cricket example About lines Line as a model: Understanding the slope Predicted values Residuals How to pick a line? Least squares criterion “Point of Means” Slope is really just correlation Variance and what happens to it Variance of original y, predicted y, residuals “R squared” Residual plots More examples
2
When x goes up 1 unit, y goes up b units.
About Lines y = mx + b y = a + bx slope = b y intercept a Slope When x goes up 1 unit, y goes up b units.
3
What can we do with the line?
Understand the relationship ( Study the slope! ) Predict values of Y based on values of X Interpolation Extrapolation
4
Extrapolation Interpolation: Using a model to estimate Y for an X value within the range on which the model was based. Extrapolation: Estimating based on an X value outside the range. Interpolation Good, Extrapolation Bad.
5
How good is the line? RESIDUALS: For each actual point, we can compute Actual Y Predicted Y Residual ( = actual minus predicted ) Small residuals GOOD No pattern in residuals GOOD
6
How to choose the line Theory: Least Squares Criterion
Choose the line with the smallest SUM OF SQUARES of the residuals. Or: Make the STANDARD DEVIATION of the residuals be as small as possible. Practice: Line of means Slope is really just r --- When X goes up one SD, Y goes up r SDs Really, b = r sy / sx
7
Linear models and non-linear models
Model A: Model B: y = a + bx + error y = a x1/2 + error Model B has smaller errors. Is it a better model?
8
aa opas asl poasie ;aaslkf 4-9043578
y = )_(*_n &*^(*LKH l;j;)(*&)(*& + error This model has even smaller errors. In fact, zero errors. Tradeoff: Small errors vs. complexity. (We’ll only consider linear models.)
9
The “Regression” Effect
A preschool program attempts to boost children’s reading scores. Children are given a pre-test and a post-test. Pre-test: mean score ≈ 100, SD ≈ 10 Post-test: mean score ≈ 100, SD ≈ 10 The program seems to have no effect.
10
A closer look at the data shows a surprising result:
Children who were below average on the pre-test tended to gain about 5-10 points on the post-test Children who were above average on the pre-test tended to lose about 5-10 points on the post-test.
11
A closer look at the data shows a surprising result:
Children who were below average on the pre-test tended to gain about 5-10 points on the post-test Children who were above average on the pre-test tended to lose about 5-10 points on the post-test. Maybe we should provide the program only for children whose pre-test scores are below average?
12
Fact: In most test–retest and analogous situations, the bottom group on the first test will on average tend to improve, while the top group on the first test will on average tend to do worse. Other examples: • Students who score high on the midterm tend on average to score high on the final, but not as high. • An athlete who has a good rookie year tends to slump in his or her second year. (“Sophomore jinx”, "Sports Illustrated Jinx") • Tall fathers tend to have sons who are tall, but not as tall. (Galton’s original example!)
14
It works the other way, too:
• Students who score high on the final tend to have scored high on the midterm, but not as high. • Tall sons tend to have fathers who are tall, but not as tall. • Students who did well on the post-test showed improvements, on average, of 5-10 points, while students who did poorly on the post-test dropped an average of 5-10 points.
15
Students can do well on the pretest…
-- because they are good readers, or -- because they get lucky. The good readers, on average, do exactly as well on the post-test. The lucky group, on average, score lower. Students can get unlucky, too, but fewer of that group are among the high-scorers on the pre-test. So the top group on the pre-test, on average, tends to score a little lower on the post-test.
Similar presentations
© 2024 SlidePlayer.com. Inc.
All rights reserved.