Download presentation
Presentation is loading. Please wait.
1
Attention I failures to select information
2
What is attention? How is the word used? Examples:
something bright caught my attention I didn’t see you, I was paying attention to the game I struggled to pay attention to the lecture I don’t remember even cleaning the table, I must not have been paying attention Attention refers to many different kinds of mechanisms Although we have an intuitive understanding of what it means to “pay attention” to an object or event, the study of attention has a long and checkered history in cognitive psychology, filled with debate and disagreement. Some have suggested that “everyone knows what attention is;” others have countered that “no one knows what attention is.” (p. 103)
3
Attention Attention enhances some information and inhibits other information. The enhancement enables us to select some information for further processing The inhibition enables us to set some information aside.
4
Attention and limits on information
We need attention to limit the amount of information that is processed Why are there limits on the amount of information we can process? limited sensory systems
5
Example 2: Fovea demo When perceiving a scene, can only get “pieces” of it at any instant; need to move eyes around to see scene Need to move eyes around to see the world seye movements make jumps called saccades
6
Example 2: Fovea demo When perceiving a scene, can only get “pieces” of it at any instant; need to move eyes around to see scene Need to move eyes around to see the world seye movements make jumps called saccades
7
Eye tracking Eye-tracking studies can tell us which information is attended to Our eyes are drawn both by top-down information (e.g. a goal to find specific information) as well as bottom-up information (e.g. a flashing light) Commercial intro on eye-tracking Fixations: ~ ms; information is acquired Saccades: extremely rapid movements between fixations Eyemovement analysis of websites can show what a user is paying attention to, and what information the user might be missing. Eye tracking device eye movements during reading
8
Eye tracking and Visual Attention
slide from Marc Pomplun: Subjects viewed ambiguous pictures that allowed two different interpretations A and B. They were asked to press and hold a particular button while perceiving interpretation A, and a different button for interpretation B. Afterwards, the fixations recorded during the perception of interpretations A and B were separated and separately visualized in the original image.
9
Eye tracking and Visual Attention
slide from Marc Pomplun: Painting by Giuseppe Arcimboldo. Earth. c Oil on wood. Private collection, Vienna, Austria.
10
[From wikipedia] http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Eye_tracking
This study by Yarbus (1967) is often referred to as evidence on how the task given to a person influences his or her eye movements. [Yarbus 1967]
11
Improving layout with eye-tracking studies
12
David Hockney’s photo collage might be a metaphor for the way we see scenes
wolfe-fig jpg 1 photo = 1 gaze
13
Attention and limits on information
Human information processing is massively parallel, up to a point where we have serial bottlenecks Bottleneck: a restriction on the amount of information that can be processed at once forcing serial processing Serial bottlenecks: limited sensory systems limited effector systems movements must be planned sequentially words can only be spoken sequentially
14
Inattentional Blindness
After bottleneck, it is the allocation of our attention that determines what is analyzed. Often, we are unable to process information that is unattended. This can lead to inattentional blindness (related to change blindness) From Simons & Chabris (1999) paper: “we often do not detect large changes to objects and scenes (`change blindness'). Furthermore, without attention, we may not even perceive objects (`inattentional blindness').”
15
Do you notice the change?
From O’Regan (1999) paper: “Change-blindness occurs when large changes are missed under natural viewing conditions because they occur simultaneously with a brief visual disruption, perhaps caused by an eye movement, a flicker, a blink, or a camera cut in a film sequence.” “Central-interest changes were usually detected as soon as they occurred, whereas marginal-interest changes were seen only on their second or later occurrences. In 13–30% of the cases, marginal-interest changes, although in full view, were not detected at all during the 40-s viewing period.” “Part of the explanation for these effects is that attention-grabbing luminance transitions, caused all over the visual field by the brief visual disruptions, prevent attention being focused on the location of the change.” “it seems that change-blindness occurs because the internal representation of the visual world is rather sparse and essentially contains only central-interest information.” Some of these demos are from: Simons & Levin, 1997, TINS, 1,
16
Do you notice the change?
Some of these demos are from: Simons & Levin, 1997, TINS, 1,
17
Do you notice the change?
Some of these demos are from: Simons & Levin, 1997, TINS, 1,
18
Do you notice the change?
Some of these demos are from: Simons & Levin, 1997, TINS, 1,
19
Other demos Lots of demos:
Airplane demo Dinner demo
20
Inattentional Blindness
Why is it hard to notice the change (initially)? When motion detection is disrupted, it is very difficult to observe changes to unattended image locations Brain makes reasonable assumption that things do not change unexpectedly (in the absence of motion cues).
21
Demo of basketball players
Task: count the number of times the white team passes the ball to each other Important to pay close attention to the white team Demos at: From Simons & Chabris (1999): “the likelihood of noticing an unexpected object depends on the similarity of that object to other objects in the display and on how difficult the priming monitoring task is. Interestingly, spatial proximity of the critical unattended object to attended locations does not appear to affect detection, suggesting that observers attend to objects and events, not spatial positions.” “Neisser and his colleagues used this `basketball-game' task [see Neisser (1979) for a description of several different versions]. In the most famous demonstration, observers attend to one team of players, pressing a key whenever one of them makes a pass, while ignoring the actions of the other team. After about 30 s, a woman carrying an open umbrella walks across the screen (this video was also superimposed on the others so all three events were partially transparent; see figure 1). She is visible for approximately 4 s before walking off the far end of the screen. The games then continue for another 25 s before the tape is stopped. Of twenty-eight naive observers, only six reported the presence of the umbrella woman, even when questioned directly after the task (Neisser and Dube, cited in Neisser 1979). Interestingly, when subjects had practice performing the task on two similar trials before the trial with the unexpected umbrella woman, 48% noticed her. When subjects just watched the screen and did not perform any task, they always noticed the umbrella woman, a result consistent with the inattentional-blindness findings reviewed earlier (and with work on saccade-contingent changes; see Grimes 1996; McConkie and Zola 1979).” “Interestingly, Neisser (1979) mentioned an additional study in which the umbrella woman wore the same-color shirt as either the attended or the unattended team. Apparently, this feature-similarity manipulation caused little difference in the rate of noticing. Also, when the unexpected character was a small boy rather than the umbrella woman, fewer subjects noticed him, and when the umbrella woman stopped her motion and performed a little dance, more subjects noticed. These latter two findings suggest that properties of an unattended stimulus can capture attention, even though similarity to attended stimuli seemed to make little difference. However, these findings must be evaluated tentatively, because the details of the experimental paradigm were not presented by Neisser (1979).” “Out of all 192 observers across all conditions, 54% noticed the unexpected event and 46% failed to notice the unexpected event, revealing a substantial level of sustained inattentional blindness for a dynamic event and confirming the basic results of Neisser and colleagues.” Did you notice anything unusual? no yes (Simons & Chabris, Perception, 1999, 28, 1059 – 1074)
22
(not) noticing person changes
Demos from Dan Simons (University of Illinois): Another demo with gender change: Failures of selection in space can be of surprising magnitude. Perhaps the most dramatic of these was a demonstration by Simons and Levin (1998) in which an experimenter stopped pedestrians on a college campus to ask for directions. During each conversation, two people carrying a door walked between the experimenter and the pedestrian. As they did, the experimenter switched places with a second experimenter who had been concealed behind the door as it was being carried. This second experimenter then continued the conversation with the pedestrian. Only half the pedestrians reported noticing the change of speaker—even when they were explicitly asked, “Did you notice that I am not the same person who first approached you to ask for directions?” (p. 105) see also letter task:
23
Inattentional Blindness
Shows there are remarkable gaps in our perception Human’s interpretation of the visual field is much sparser than the subjective experience of “seeing” suggests Our visual system might be overwhelmed without change blindness -- in a real-world setting with many moving objects, it might make sense to “track” only a few objects From O’Regan et al. (1999) paper: “These results indicate that humans’ internal representation of the visual field is much sparser than the subjective experience of ‘seeing’ suggests. Only the parts of the environment that observers attend to and encode as interesting are available for making comparisons.” “If only attended parts of the environment are represented in the brain, how can we have the impression of such richness and completeness in the visual world outside us? The answer might be that the visual world acts as an external memory. We have the impression of simultaneously seeing everything, because any portion of the visual field that awakens our interest is immediately available for scrutiny through an unconscious flick of the eye or of attention. However, those parts of the scene that are not being currently processed (and are in some sense not ‘seen’) nevertheless constitute a background or setting that enlivens our visual experience. The idea of the world as an outside memory is receiving attention from scientists and philosophers interested in the problem of perceptual ‘filling in’ of visual scotomas and illusory contours. Work in robotics with the concept of ‘active’ vision is also adopting the notion that using the outside world to represent information might be more efficient than making an internal copy.” From Simons & Chabris (1999): “The richness of our visual experience leads us to believe that our visual representations will include and preserve the same amount of detail”
24
Failures of Selection in Time
When new information (even if only a small amount) arrives in a rapid stream, spending time processing it will cause you to miss some other incoming information, resulting in what are called failures of selection in time.
25
Attentional Blink From Wikipedia:
“One curious aspect of attentional blink is that it usually includes "lag 1 sparing", meaning that targets presented very close together in time (at "lag 1" or consecutively in the RSVP stream) are not affected by the attentional blink, even though items presented at slightly greater lags are significantly impaired. There is as yet no conclusive explanation for the phenomenon of lag 1 sparing, although it is thought to be related to the first parallel stage of the two-stage system of stimulus selection and processing.” “One mechanism that may give rise to the attentional blink is that both targets (T1 and T2) can be processed in parallel if presented close enough together; otherwise serial processing takes over, which creates a temporary bottleneck for subsequent processing of T2 from visual short-term memory. An alternative account posits that the bottleneck occurs in a limited-capacity processing stage required for awareness and preparation of a response. There are also hybrid models that incorporate both of these ideas.” Demos:
26
Sources of Limitation The attentional blink is a short period during which incoming information is not registered, similar in effect to the physical blanking out of visual information during the blink of an eye. Divided-attention studies demonstrate that performance is hampered when you have to attend to two separate sources of visual information or two separate visual events. In all these cases, the decrement in performance is referred to as dual-task interference. The interference, in the form of a slowing down of your actions, that arises when you try to select between two possible responses to even a sole sensory stimulus is referred to as a response bottleneck. The additional time needed to move through this kind of bottleneck has been measured experimentally. (p. 115)
Similar presentations
© 2025 SlidePlayer.com. Inc.
All rights reserved.