Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Class 1a. CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Some things we know Is this English? Is this English? Pat the book lifted. Pat the book lifted. Pat lifted the book. Pat.

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Class 1a. CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Some things we know Is this English? Is this English? Pat the book lifted. Pat the book lifted. Pat lifted the book. Pat."— Presentation transcript:

1 Class 1a. CAS LX 522 Syntax I

2 Some things we know Is this English? Is this English? Pat the book lifted. Pat the book lifted. Pat lifted the book. Pat lifted the book. Lifted Pat the book. Lifted Pat the book. Pat book the lifted. Pat book the lifted. Why? Why?

3 Some things we know Asterisks = don’t qualify as English Asterisks = don’t qualify as English *Pat the book lifted. *Pat the book lifted. Pat lifted the book. Pat lifted the book. *Lifted Pat the book. *Lifted Pat the book. *Pat book the lifted. *Pat book the lifted. Why? We know a grammar of English, a system differentiating grammatical from ungrammatical sentences. Why? We know a grammar of English, a system differentiating grammatical from ungrammatical sentences.

4 (Un)acceptability *Big that under staple run the jump swim. *Big that under staple run the jump swim. My toothbrush is pregnant again. My toothbrush is pregnant again. As I knitted the sock fell to the floor. As I knitted the sock fell to the floor. The rat the cat the dog chased caught escaped adeptly. The rat the cat the dog chased caught escaped adeptly.

5 Ambiguity and stars I sat by the bank. I sat by the bank. How did John say Mary fixed the car? How did John say Mary fixed the car? With a wrench. With a wrench. In a high-pitched voice. In a high-pitched voice. How did John ask if Mary fixed the car? How did John ask if Mary fixed the car? *With a wrench. *With a wrench. In a high-pitched voice. In a high-pitched voice.

6 Parentheses and optionality Pat (quickly) ran to the bank. Pat (quickly) ran to the bank. Pat ran to the bank. Pat ran to the bank. Pat quickly ran to the bank. Pat quickly ran to the bank. Pat washed (*quickly) the asparagus. Pat washed (*quickly) the asparagus. Pat washed the asparagus Pat washed the asparagus *Pat washed quickly the asparagus. *Pat washed quickly the asparagus. The dish ran away with *(the) spoon. The dish ran away with *(the) spoon. The dish ran away with the spoon. The dish ran away with the spoon. *The dish ran away with spoon. *The dish ran away with spoon. I saw (a/the/*quickly) book on the shelf. I saw (a/the/*quickly) book on the shelf. I saw a book on the shelf. I saw a book on the shelf. I saw the book on the shelf. I saw the book on the shelf. *I saw quickly book on the shelf. *I saw quickly book on the shelf.

7 Our knowledge is surprisingly complicated 1) Tony threw out the couch. 2) Tony threw the couch out.  Prepositions can go on either side of the object? 3) Tony stormed out the door. 4) * Tony stormed the door out.

8 Our knowledge is surprisingly complicated 5) What did Mary say Pat bought? 6) What did Mary say that Pat bought?  Ok, that is optional? 7) Who did Mary say bought coffee? 8) *Who did Mary say that bought coffee?

9 Our knowledge is surprisingly complicated 9) Bill thinks Mary is a genius. 10) Her mother thinks Mary is a genius. 11) She thinks Mary is a genius. 12) I asked Mary to buy coffee. 13) What did you ask Mary to buy? 14) Who did you say bought coffee? 15) I borrowed the book Bill bought in DC. 16) *Who did you borrow the book bought in DC?

10 How do people know this? All native speakers of English know this. All native speakers of English know this. Little kids weren’t told these rules (or punished for violating them)… Little kids weren’t told these rules (or punished for violating them)… “You can’t question a subject in a complement embedded with that” “You can’t question a subject in a complement embedded with that” “You can’t use a proper name as an object if the subject is co-referential.” “You can’t use a proper name as an object if the subject is co-referential.”

11 Two questions What do people know about their language? What do people know about their language? Including things we know “unconsciously” Including things we know “unconsciously” How do people come to know it? How do people come to know it? Tricky question for things that we don’t know we know. Tricky question for things that we don’t know we know.

12 Systematicity What people eventually end up with is a system with which they can produce (and rate) sentences. A grammar. What people eventually end up with is a system with which they can produce (and rate) sentences. A grammar. Even if you’ve never heard these before, you know which one is “English” and which one isn’t: Even if you’ve never heard these before, you know which one is “English” and which one isn’t: 17) Eight very lazy elephants drank brandy. 18) Eight elephants very lazy brandy drank. Kids say wugs. Kids say wugs.

13 Positive and negative evidence Adults know if a given sentence S is grammatical or ungrammatical. This is part of the knowledge kids gain through language acquisition. Adults know if a given sentence S is grammatical or ungrammatical. This is part of the knowledge kids gain through language acquisition. Kids hear grammatical sentences (positive evidence) Kids hear grammatical sentences (positive evidence) Kids are not generally told which sentences are ungrammatical (no negative evidence) Kids are not generally told which sentences are ungrammatical (no negative evidence)

14 Positive and negative evidence One of the striking things about child language is how few errors they actually make. One of the striking things about child language is how few errors they actually make. For negative feedback to work, the kids have to make the errors (so that it can get the negative response). For negative feedback to work, the kids have to make the errors (so that it can get the negative response). But they don’t make the errors. But they don’t make the errors. (Kids do make errors, but not of the kind that one might expect if they were just trying to extract patterns from the language data they hear) (Kids do make errors, but not of the kind that one might expect if they were just trying to extract patterns from the language data they hear)

15 Poverty of the stimulus What is the next number in this sequence? What is the next number in this sequence? 1, 2, 3, __ 1, 2, 3, __ How do you form a yes-no question? How do you form a yes-no question? Pat will leave. Pat will leave. Will Pat leave? Will Pat leave? The book that you were reading was good. The book that you were reading was good. *Book the that you were reading was good? *Book the that you were reading was good? *Were the book that you reading was good? *Were the book that you reading was good? Was the book that you were reading good? Was the book that you were reading good?

16 The “Language instinct” The linguistic capacity is part of being human. The linguistic capacity is part of being human. Like having two arms, ten fingers, a vision system, humans have a language faculty. Like having two arms, ten fingers, a vision system, humans have a language faculty. The language faculty (tightly) constrains what kinds of languages a child can learn. The language faculty (tightly) constrains what kinds of languages a child can learn. =“Universal Grammar” (UG). =“Universal Grammar” (UG).

17 But languages differ English, French: Subject Verb Object (SVO) English, French: Subject Verb Object (SVO) John ate an apple. John ate an apple. Pierre a mangé une pomme. Pierre a mangé une pomme. Japanese, Korean: Subject Object Verb (SOV) Japanese, Korean: Subject Object Verb (SOV) Taroo-wa ringo-o tabeta. Taroo-wa ringo-o tabeta. Chelswu-ka sakwa-lul mekessta. Chelswu-ka sakwa-lul mekessta. Irish, Arabic (VSO), Malagasy (VOS), … Irish, Arabic (VSO), Malagasy (VOS), …

18 But languages differ English: Adverbs before verbs English: Adverbs before verbs Mary quickly eats an apple. Mary quickly eats an apple. (also: Mary ate an apple quickly) (also: Mary ate an apple quickly) *Mary eats quickly an apple. *Mary eats quickly an apple. French: Adverbs after verbs French: Adverbs after verbs Geneviève mange rapidement une pomme. Geneviève mange rapidement une pomme. *Geneviève rapidement mange une pomme. *Geneviève rapidement mange une pomme.

19 Parameters We can categorize languages in terms of their word order: SVO, SOV, VSO. We can categorize languages in terms of their word order: SVO, SOV, VSO. This is a parameter by which languages differ. This is a parameter by which languages differ. The dominant formal theory of first language acquisition holds that children have access to a set of parameters by which languages can differ; acquisition is the process of setting those parameters. The dominant formal theory of first language acquisition holds that children have access to a set of parameters by which languages can differ; acquisition is the process of setting those parameters. What are the parameters? What are the parameters? What are the “universal” principles of grammar? What are the “universal” principles of grammar?

20 The enterprise The data we will primarily be concerned with are native speaker intuitions. The data we will primarily be concerned with are native speaker intuitions. Native speakers, faced with a sentence S, know whether the sentence S is part of their language or isn’t. These intuitions are highly systematic. Native speakers, faced with a sentence S, know whether the sentence S is part of their language or isn’t. These intuitions are highly systematic. We want to uncover the system (which is unconscious knowledge) behind the intuitions of native speakers—their knowledge of language. We want to uncover the system (which is unconscious knowledge) behind the intuitions of native speakers—their knowledge of language.

21 I-language We are studying the system behind one person’s pattern of intuitions. We are studying the system behind one person’s pattern of intuitions. Speakers growing up in the same community have very similar knowledge, but language is an individual thing (“I-language”). Speakers growing up in the same community have very similar knowledge, but language is an individual thing (“I-language”). One doesn’t need to ask the Académie française whether Geneviève rapidement mange une pomme is a sentence of French. One knows. One doesn’t need to ask the Académie française whether Geneviève rapidement mange une pomme is a sentence of French. One knows. I-languages of a community is can be characterized, but it is external to the speaker (“E- language”), not any one person’s knowledge, a generalization over many people’s I-languages. I-languages of a community is can be characterized, but it is external to the speaker (“E- language”), not any one person’s knowledge, a generalization over many people’s I-languages. For example, Parisian French. For example, Parisian French.

22 Competence We are also concerned with what a person knows. What characterizes a person’s language competence. We are in general not concerned here with how a person ends up using this knowledge (performance). We are also concerned with what a person knows. What characterizes a person’s language competence. We are in general not concerned here with how a person ends up using this knowledge (performance). You still have your language competence when you are sleeping, in the absence of any performance. Being drunk doesn’t make one think “bought some John coffee” is English, though perhaps one might say it. You still have your language competence when you are sleeping, in the absence of any performance. Being drunk doesn’t make one think “bought some John coffee” is English, though perhaps one might say it.

23 Prescriptive rules Another thing we need to be cautious of are prescriptive rules. Often prescriptive rules of “good grammar” turn out to be impositions on our native grammar which run counter to our native competence. Another thing we need to be cautious of are prescriptive rules. Often prescriptive rules of “good grammar” turn out to be impositions on our native grammar which run counter to our native competence. After all, why did they need to be rules in the first place? After all, why did they need to be rules in the first place?

24 Prescriptive rules Prepositions are things you don’t end a sentence with. Prepositions are things you don’t end a sentence with. It is important to religiously avoid splitting infinitives. It is important to religiously avoid splitting infinitives. Remember: Capitalize the first word after a colon. Remember: Capitalize the first word after a colon. Don’t be so immodest as to say I and John left; say John and I left instead. Don’t be so immodest as to say I and John left; say John and I left instead. Impact is not a verb. Impact is not a verb. The book which you just bought is offensive. The book which you just bought is offensive.

25 Prescriptive rules When making grammaticality judgments (or when asking others to make grammaticality judgments), we must do our best to factor out prescriptive rules (learned explicitly, e.g., in school). When making grammaticality judgments (or when asking others to make grammaticality judgments), we must do our best to factor out prescriptive rules (learned explicitly, e.g., in school). We’re not interested in studying the prescriptive rules; we could just look them up, and it isn’t likely to tell us anything deep about the makeup of the human mind. They’re really just a “secret handshake,” allowing educated people to detect one another. We’re not interested in studying the prescriptive rules; we could just look them up, and it isn’t likely to tell us anything deep about the makeup of the human mind. They’re really just a “secret handshake,” allowing educated people to detect one another.

26 Syntax as science Syntax, as practiced here, is a scientific enterprise. This means, in particular, approaching syntax using the scientific method. Syntax, as practiced here, is a scientific enterprise. This means, in particular, approaching syntax using the scientific method. Step 1: Gather observations (data) Step 1: Gather observations (data) Step 2: Make generalizations Step 2: Make generalizations Step 3: Form hypotheses Step 3: Form hypotheses Step 4: Test predictions made by these hypotheses, returning to step 1. Step 4: Test predictions made by these hypotheses, returning to step 1.

27 Syntax as science This is pretty much the way other scientific disciplines work… biology, chemistry, physics. This is pretty much the way other scientific disciplines work… biology, chemistry, physics. We may start out with a kind of “folk understanding” of a field. We may start out with a kind of “folk understanding” of a field. For example, you push something and it moves. You stop pushing, and it stops. The sun revolves around the earth from East to West, followed by the moon. Water is a basic element, like fire. Whales are very big fish, like dolphins, or tuna, but bigger. For example, you push something and it moves. You stop pushing, and it stops. The sun revolves around the earth from East to West, followed by the moon. Water is a basic element, like fire. Whales are very big fish, like dolphins, or tuna, but bigger. Ockham’s Razor: posit as few concepts and relations as we can get away with. A leaner theory is a better theory. A more easily falsifiable theory is a better theory too. Ockham’s Razor: posit as few concepts and relations as we can get away with. A leaner theory is a better theory. A more easily falsifiable theory is a better theory too.

28 Levels of adequacy If our hypotheses can predict the existence of the grammatical sentences in a corpus (a set of grammatical sentences), it is observationally adequate. If our hypotheses can predict the existence of the grammatical sentences in a corpus (a set of grammatical sentences), it is observationally adequate. If our hypotheses can predict the native- speaker intuitions about which sentences are grammatical and which are ungrammatical, it is descriptively adequate. If our hypotheses can predict the native- speaker intuitions about which sentences are grammatical and which are ungrammatical, it is descriptively adequate.

29 Levels of adequacy If we can take a descriptively adequate set of hypotheses one step further and account not only for the native speaker judgments but also for how children come to have these judgments, our hypotheses are explanatorily adequate. If we can take a descriptively adequate set of hypotheses one step further and account not only for the native speaker judgments but also for how children come to have these judgments, our hypotheses are explanatorily adequate. It’s this last level that we are hoping to achieve. It’s this last level that we are hoping to achieve. Basic principles Basic principles Parameters of variation Parameters of variation How to set the parameters from child’s input How to set the parameters from child’s input

30 Infinite use of finite means English has an infinite number of sentences. Any natural language does. English has an infinite number of sentences. Any natural language does. John said that English has an infinite number of sentences. John said that English has an infinite number of sentences. Mary said that John said that English has an infinite number of sentences. Mary said that John said that English has an infinite number of sentences. Pat said that Mary said that John said that English has an infinite number of sentences. Pat said that Mary said that John said that English has an infinite number of sentences. Tracy said that Pat said that Mary said that John said that English has an infinite number of sentences. Tracy said that Pat said that Mary said that John said that English has an infinite number of sentences. Chris said that Tracy said that Pat said that Mary said that John said that English has an infinite number of sentences. Chris said that Tracy said that Pat said that Mary said that John said that English has an infinite number of sentences. If S is a sentence and N is a name, N said that S is also a sentence. If S is a sentence and N is a name, N said that S is also a sentence. S  N said that S S  N said that S Some of the earliest work in grammatical theory was done by trying to state rules of this form, the goal being to generate the sentences of a language. Some of the earliest work in grammatical theory was done by trying to state rules of this form, the goal being to generate the sentences of a language.

31 Of the past and the future Serious scientific study of sentence structure of this kind generally began in the 50’s, driven to a great extent by the work of Noam Chomsky. Serious scientific study of sentence structure of this kind generally began in the 50’s, driven to a great extent by the work of Noam Chomsky. It’s now half a century later, and we have learned a lot about how syntax works. It’s now half a century later, and we have learned a lot about how syntax works.

32 Of the past and the future Progress was incremental, and often required revising our assumptions about how sentences are really put together. Progress was incremental, and often required revising our assumptions about how sentences are really put together. Data was examined, generalizations were arrived at, hypotheses were formed, predictions were tested—and often led to revisions of the generalizations and the hypotheses, and so forth. Data was examined, generalizations were arrived at, hypotheses were formed, predictions were tested—and often led to revisions of the generalizations and the hypotheses, and so forth.

33 Of the past and the future Two goals of the class: Two goals of the class: Think like a syntactician. Think like a syntactician. Be able to read (relatively recent) books, articles, etc. about syntax. Be able to read (relatively recent) books, articles, etc. about syntax. It’s not really enough to just know what people concluded, we need to understand why they concluded what they did. It’s not really enough to just know what people concluded, we need to understand why they concluded what they did.

34 Some milestones Until about the mid-70’s, phrase structure rules. Until about the mid-70’s, phrase structure rules. S  NP VPVP  V (NP) Mid-70’s, X-Bar Theory (a generalization about what are possible PSRs). Mid-70’s, X-Bar Theory (a generalization about what are possible PSRs). In the 80’s, a fairly significant shift to Government and Binding Theory (viewing grammar a little less like a computer program). Very productive. In the 80’s, a fairly significant shift to Government and Binding Theory (viewing grammar a little less like a computer program). Very productive. In the 90’s, another shift to the Minimalist Program (an attempt at simplification, as well as a change in philosophy). In the 90’s, another shift to the Minimalist Program (an attempt at simplification, as well as a change in philosophy).

35                       


Download ppt "Class 1a. CAS LX 522 Syntax I. Some things we know Is this English? Is this English? Pat the book lifted. Pat the book lifted. Pat lifted the book. Pat."

Similar presentations


Ads by Google