Download presentation
Presentation is loading. Please wait.
Published byBeryl Ross Modified over 9 years ago
1
1.Today: Review of material for the exam (chapters 9,10,&13) 1.Dec. 14: Exam 3 grades posted; 2.Dec. 15: Final grades posted; Exam: Multiple choice questions; Problems (2-3 per chapter); Information/preparation: http://www.colorado.edu/physics/phys1230/phys1230_fa11/Exams.htm Practicing problems; Reading Material; Solutions will be posted on the web page soon after the exam;
2
The same color sensation can often be produced by 2 or more different intensity distribution curves Here is an intensity distribution curve which gives us the sensation of yellow Here is a different intensity distribution curve which also gives us the same sensation of yellow The two colors described by the two different intenstiy curves are called metamers
3
Hue, Saturation and Brightness (HSB): One way to use 3 numbers to specify a color instead of using an intensity-distribution curve Color tree (e.g. Fig. 9.5 in book) Moving up the tree increases the lightness of a color Moving around a circle of given radius changes the hue of a color Moving along a radius of a circle changes the saturation (vividness) of a color These three coordinates can be described in terms of three numbers Photoshop: uses H, S and B lightness hue saturation
5
Red, green and blue (RGB): RGB is another way to use 3 numbers to specify a color instead of using an intensity-distribution curve or HSB In addition to using Hue, Saturation and Brightness (HSB); Many (but not all) colors can be described in terms of the relative intensities of a light mixture of a certain wavelength red, wavelength green and wavelength blue lights 650-nm red 530-nm green 460-nm blue These are called the additive primaries The mixing of the additive primaries is called additive mixing Additive mixing is usually done by mixing primary color lights with different intensities but there are other ways to be discussed later Demonstrate with Physics 2000 cyan magenta yellow 650-nm red 530-nm green 460-nm blue http://www.colorado.edu/physics/ 2000/tv/colortv.html
6
Complementary additive colors Definition of complementary color (for additive mixtures): The complement of a color is a second color. When the second color is additively mixed to the first, the result is white. Blue & yellow are complementary B + Y = W. Green & magenta are complementary G + M = W Cyan and red are complementary C + R = W Magenta is not a wavelength color— it is not in the rainbow There is at most one wavelength complementary color for each wavelength color (Fig 9.9) white cyan red magentagreen yellow blue
7
Additive mixing of colored light primaries Blue added to green = cyan. Green added to red = yellow. Red added to blue = magenta.
8
Complementary colored lights (additive mixing) Blue (primary) and yellow. Green (primary) and magenta. Red (primary) and cyan.
9
Chromaticity diagrams: Yet another way to represent colors by (3) numbers The chromaticity diagram is in many ways similar to a color tree A chromaticity diagram has a fixed brightness or lightness for all colors Wavelength colors are on the horseshoe rim but non-wavelength colors like magenta are on the flat part of the rim Inside are the less saturated colors, including white at the interior less saturated colors saturated wavelength colors saturated non-wavelength colors
10
Using the chromaticity diagram to identify colors The numbers that we use to identify a color are its x-value and y-value inside the diagram and a z-value to indicate its brightness or lightness x and y specify the chromaticity of a color Example: Apple pickers are told around the country that certain apples are best picked when they are a certaim red (see black dot) Since the chromaticity diagram is a world standard the company can tell its employees to pick when the apples have chromaticity x = 0.57 y = 0.28 The "purest" white is at x = 0.33 and y = 0.33 Chromaticity diagram can be related to colors in Photoshop
11
Using the chromaticity diagram to understand the result of additive mixing of colors An additive mixture of two wavelength colors lies along the line joining them Example: The colors seen by mixing 700 nm red and 500 nm green lie along the line shown Where along the line is the color of the mixture? Answer depends on the relative intensities of the 700 nm red and the 500 nm green. Here is what you get when the green is much more intense than the red (a green) Here is what you get when the red is much more intense than the green (a red) Here is what you get when the red is slightly more intense than the green (a yellow) Note — this works for adding two colors in middle also!
12
Using the chromaticity diagram to understand complementary colors The complement to any wavelength color on the edge of the chromaticity diagram is obtained by drawing a straight line from that color through white to the other edge of the diagram Example: The complement to 700 nm red is 490 nm cyan Example: The complement to green is magenta - a non- wavelength color
13
Using the chromaticity diagram to find the dominant hue of a color in the interior of the diagram To find the dominant hue of the color indicated by the black dot Draw st. line from white through the point to get dominant wavelength, and hence, hue (547 nm green) Works because additive mixture of white with a fully-saturated (wavelength) color gives the desaturated color of the original point
14
Partitive mixing is another kind of additive color mixing but not achieved by superimposing colored lights! Instead, it works by putting small patches of colors next to each other. From a distance these colors mix just as though they were colored lights superimposed on each other Examples: Seurat pointillism Color TV and computer screens (Physics 2000) Photoshop example What is partitive mixing?
16
A colored filter subtracts colors by absorption. = Incident white light Only green gets through Cyan filter subtracts red Yellow filter subtracts blue
17
A colored filter subtracts certain colors by absorption and transmits the rest = Incident white light Magenta filter subtracts green Cyan filter subtracts red Only blue gets through
18
A colored filter subtracts colors by absorption. = Incident white light Magenta filter subtracts green Only red gets through Yellow filter subtracts blue
20
What is the effect of combining (sandwiching) different colored filters together? Rules for combining the subtractive primaries, cyan, yellow and magenta: White light passed through a cyan filter plus a magenta filter appears blue White light passed through a yellow filter plus a magenta filter appears red White light passed through a yellow filter plus a cyan filter appears green Why? cyan magenta yellow
21
Colored surfaces subtract certain colors by absorbing them, while reflecting others Magenta surface absorbs (subtracts) green. Green surface absorbs (subtracts) red and blue (magenta). White in Magenta out White in Green out
22
Green light on a magenta surface appears colorless because green is absorbed Magenta surface absorbs (subtracts) green. Green surface absorbs (subtracts) red and blue (magenta). Magenta light on a green surface appears colorless because magenta is absorbed Green in No color Magenta in No color
23
When looking at a colored object in a colored light source what is the resulting color? Rule: Multiply the intensity-distribution of the light source by the reflectance of the colored object to get the intensity distribution of the the illuminated object Example: Look at a magenta shirt in reflected light from a Cool White fluorescent tube. It appears grey (colorless) Confirm by multiplying the intensity distribution curve by the reflectance curve to get the new intensity distribution curve for the reflected light Cool white fluorescent bulb Magenta shirt You multiply the two y-values at each x to get the new curve this number equals this number This number times How the shirt appears in this light
24
Halftone Left: Halftone dots. Right: How the human eye would see this sort of arrangement from a sufficient distance or when they are small. Resolution: m easured in lines per inch (lpi) or dots per inch (dpi); for example, Laser Printer (600dpi)lines per inch
25
Color halftoning Three examples of color halftoning with CMYK separations. From left to right: The cyan separation, the magenta separation, the yellow separation, the black separation, the combined halftone pattern and finally how the human eye would observe the combined halftone pattern from a sufficient distance. Paper beneath Printer's ink
26
Demonstration
27
Color Liquid Crystal Displays (LCDs)
28
Chapter 10: We have three different kinds of cones whose responses are mainly at short, intermediate and long wavelengths s-cones absorb short wavelength light best, with peak response at 450 nm (blue) L-cones absorb long wavelength light best, with peak response at 580 nm (red) i-cones absorb intermediate wavelengths best, with peak response at 540 nm (green) Light at any wavelength in the visual spectrum from 400 to 700 nm will excite these 3 types of cones to a degree depending on the intensity at each wavelength. Our perception of which color we are seeing (color sensation) is determined by how much S, i and L resonse occurs to light of a particular intensity distribution. Rule: To get the overall response of each type of cone, multiply the intensity of the light at each wavelength by the response of the cone at that wavelength and then add together all of the products for all of the wavenumbers in the intensity distribution i-cones L-cones s-cones Spectral response of cones in typical human eye relative response
29
Examples of two different ways we see white Spectral response of cones in typical human eye relative response Our sensation of color depends on how much total s, i & L cone response occurs due to a light intensity- distribution Multiply the intensity distribution curve by each response curve to determine how much total S, i, and L response occurs We experience the sensation white when we have equal total s, i & L responses There are many ways this can occur!! E.g., when broadband light enters our eye Another way to experience white is by viewing a mixture of blue and yellow E.g., 460 nm blue of intensity 1 and 575 nm yellow of intensity 1.66 The blue excites mainly s-cones but also a bit of i-cones and a bit of L-cones The yellow excites i-cones and (slightly more) L-cones but no s-cones The result is an equal response of s-cones, i- cones and L-cones (details) 575 nm yellow of intensity 1.66 460 nm blue of intensity 1 1 1.66 0
30
How does a normal person see yellow when only red and green lights are superimposed? Spectral response of cones in typical human eye relative response Our sensation of yellow depends on a special s, i & L cone response We experience the sensation yellow when 575 nm light reaches our eyes What really gives us the sensation of yellow is the almost equal response of i and L cones together with no s-cones!! Another way to experience yellow is by seeing overlapping red & green lights E.g., 530 nm green of intensity 1 and 650 nm red of intensity 2.15 The green excites mainly i-cones but also L-cones, while the red excites mainly L- cones but also i-cones The total respone of s & i-cones due to the spectral green and red is the same as the total response due to spectral yellow In general need 3 wavelength lights to mix to any color 575 nm yellow of intensity 1.35 530 nm green of intensity 1 1 2.15 0 650 nm red of intensity 2.15 Light colorBrightness S-cone response I-cone responseL-cone response 530 nm green1negligible4128 650 nm red2.15negligible 2.15 x 22.15 x 9 Mixture yellow (perceived as yellow )negligible41 +2.15 x 2 =4528 +2.15 x 9 =47 575 nm yellow 1.35negligible1.35 x 33 = 451.35 x 35 = 47
31
We can verify color naming of hues in terms of the psychological primaries on the chromaticity diagram All of the hues can be named qualitatively by how much green, red, blue or yellow is "in" them We don't need orange, purple or pink: orange can be thought of as yellow-red purple can be thought of as red-blue pink has the same hue as red but differs only in lightness We can break up the diagram into 4 different regions by drawing two lines whose endpoints are the psychological primary hues The endpoints of the yellow line are 580 nm "unique" yellow and 475 nm "unique" blue magentaOne endpoint of the red line is 500 nm "unique" green and the other is "red" (not unique or spectral - really more like magenta) Greenness & yellowness Redness & yellowness blueness Greenness & blueness
32
What is meant by the opponent nature of red vs green (r-g) perception and of yellow vs blue (y-b) perception. Viewing a progression of colors in the direction of the yellow line from 475 nm blue towards 580 nm yellow, we see more yellowness of each color and less blueness. We call this perception our y-b channel Yellow & blue are opponents Moving parallel to the red line from 500 nm green towards nonspectral red we see more redness in each color and less greenness. We call this perception our r-g channel Red and green are opponents The lines cross at white, where both y-b & r-g are neutralized Greenness & yellowness Redness & yellowness blueness Greenness & blueness y-b r-g
33
How might the three types of cones be "wired" to neural cells to account for our perception of hues in terms of two opponent pairs of psychological primaries r-g and y-b? The 3 kinds of cones are related to r-g and y-b by the way they are connected to neural cells (such as ganglion cells) Cones of each kind are attached to 3 different neural cells which control the two chromatic channels, y-b and r-g, and the white vs black channel called the achromatic channel (lightness) "wiring" is the following: When light falls on the L-cones they tell all 3 neural cells to increase the electrical signal they send to the brain When light falls on the i-cones they tell the r-g channel cell to decrease (inhibit) its signal but tell the other cells to increase their signal When light falls on the s-cones they tell the y-b channel cell to decrease (inhibit) its signal but tell the other cells to increse their signal s-conei-coneL-cone neural cell for y-b chromatic channel neural cell for r-g chromatic channel neural cell for w-blk achromatic channel Electrical signal to brain + + + + + + +
34
s-conei-coneL-cone neural cell for y-b chromatic channel neural cell for r-g chromatic channel neural cell for w-blk achromatic channel Electrical signal to brain ++ ++ + ++ How can this "wiring" work to produce the chromatic channels? The neural cell for the y-b chromatic channel has its signal inhibited when (bluE) light excites the s-cone INTERPRETED AS BLUE enhanced when light excites the i & L cones INTERPRETED AS YELLOW The neural cell for the r-g chromatic channel has its signal inhibited when (green) light falls on the i-cone INTERPRETED AS GREEN enhanced when light excites the s and L cone INTERPRETED AS MAGENTA (Psychological red) The neural cell for the achromatic channel has its signal enhanced when light excites any of the cones
35
Systematic description of color-blindness (no need to memorize terminology) Monochromacy (can match any colored light with any 1 spectral light by adjusting intensity) Either has no cones (rod monochromat) or has only 1 of the 3 types of cones working (cone monochromat). Sees ony whites, greys, blacks, no hues Dichromacy (can match any colored light with 2 spectral lights of different intensities of (rather than the normal 3) L-cone function lacking = protanopia i-cone function lacking = deuteranopia s-cone function lacking = tritanopia no y-b channel but all 3 cones OK = tetartanopia Anomalous trichromacy (can match any colored light with 3 spectral lights of different intensities as in normal vision, but still have color perception problems) Protanomaly Shifted L-cone response curve Deuteranomaly (most common) Shifted i-cone response curve Confusion between red and green. Tritanomaly Yellow-blue problems: probably defective s-cones Neuteranomaly ineffective r-g channel
36
Receptive field of a double-opponent cell of the r-g type 2 different ways to INCREASE the signal the ganglion cell sends to brain2 different ways to INCREASE the signal the ganglion cell sends to brain RedcenterRed light falling on cones in center of receptive field attached to ganglion cell GreensurroundGreen light on surround 2 different ways to the signal the ganglion cell sends to the brain2 different ways to decrease the signal the ganglion cell sends to the brain RedsurroundRed light on surround Green light on centerGreen light on center Electrical signal to brain from ganglion cell is at ambient level when no light is on center or surround redWhen signal to brain is INCREASEDwe interpret that as red greenWhen signal to brain is decreased we interpret that as green signal to brain
37
We can summarize this by just showing the center & surround of the receptive field and indicating the effect of red (R) and green (G) on each A double-opponent cell differs from a single opponent cell In both of them R in the center increases the signal In a single-opponent cell G in surround would inhibit signal, whereas in double-opponent cell G enhances In a double-opponent cell R in center enhances signal (ganglion cell signals red) G in surround enhances signal (ganglion cell signals red) R in surround inhibits signal (ganglion cell signals green) G in center inhibits signal (ganglion cell signals green) Fictional cell real cell
38
How do 3D movies use polaroid filters?
Similar presentations
© 2024 SlidePlayer.com. Inc.
All rights reserved.