Download presentation
Presentation is loading. Please wait.
Published byMargaret McDowell Modified over 9 years ago
1
Perception
2
It’s not as Easy as it Looks
3
Sensation and Perception Sensation The process through which the senses pick up visual, auditory, and other sensory stimuli and transmit them to the brain; sensory information that has registered in the brain but has not been interpreted Perception The process by which sensory information is actively organized and interpreted by the brain
4
We see the world with our brain How Light Enters the Eye
5
Interpretation What do you see?
6
Interpretation Now what do you see?
7
Attention and Perception Your perception of a rich visual environ- ment is an illusion! You actually are only aware of the small part of the scene that you are attending to
8
Attention and Perception Change Blindness/Inattentive Blindness you can’t notice changes in a scene unless you attend to the location of the change Bet you can't do this
9
Illusions An illusion occurs when what you perceive is not what is really out there Optical illusions occur when the physical stimulus itself is distorted – but interpreted correctly by the brain! But sometimes the brain gets it wrong – and we see a visual illusion Visual Illusions happen in your visual system – the physical stimulus is correct but misinterpreted by the brain.
10
Optical and visual illusions Optical IllusionVisual Illusion
11
Visual illusion
12
Categories of visual illusion Gregory identified FOUR categories of visual illusion. Ambiguous figures Paradoxical figures Fictitious figures Distortions
13
Ambiguous figures the same input leads to different outputs due to switches in attention. For these figures we make two alternative hypotheses about what sort of object could result in that particular pattern of information on our retina. We can only fulfil one of these hypotheses at a time, but it depends which one
14
Ambiguous figures – 1 Necker Cube In which direction is the cube facing?
15
Ambiguous figures – 2 Rubin vase
16
Ambiguous figures – 3
17
Paradoxical figures Figures we assume are “real” 3-D objects are impossible in the “real world”. We appear to be unable to accept that they are simply lines drawn on a flat surface, in two dimensions Our hypothesis appears to be that there are a number of depth cues in the drawings, so they must represent 3-D objects and we attempt to interpret the objects in three dimensions.
18
Paradoxical figures - Impossible Staircase
19
Fictitious figures We see what is not there, not actually given in the stimulus array. We appear to construct perceptual hypotheses based on our best guess about the whole visual array, which gives us a perception of an object that is not actually there!
20
Fictitious figures - Kanizsa triangle
21
Distortions Here we make a perceptual mistake We attempt to understand the data in terms of how we normally interpret the world but this misleads us and we make mistakes. It seems our perception is greatly affected by the context in which an object is seen - we make a hypothesis based on what we normally experience in these circumstances and that hypothesis is mistaken.
22
Distortions – 1 Muller-Lyle
23
Explanation? Gregory – we impose our knowledge of a 3- D world onto a 2-D image. Day – ‘conflicting cues theory’ i.e. we have to make sense of 2 cues when judging the length of the lines – the actual length of the line and the overall length of the figure.
24
Distortions – 2 Ponzo
25
Ponzo
26
Distortions – 3 Titchener Another example of a distortion illusion is Titchener’s circles
27
Explanation Your brain usually tries to increase differences between things to make it easier to tell them apart. In the left hand image to make it even easier for you to tell the large outer circles apart from the smaller middle circle, your brain makes the difference between them even larger. This makes the middle circle look even smaller. In the right hand image to make it even easier for you to tell the small outer circles apart from the larger middle circle, your brain makes the difference between them even larger. This makes the middle circle look even bigger. Comparing the middle circles, one looks larger than it really is while the other one looks smaller than it really is.
28
You decide On the next slides decide which type of illusion is being created!
29
Penrose Triangle Ambiguous? Paradoxical? Fictitious? Distortion?
30
Ambihelical Hexnut Ambiguous? Paradoxical? Fictitious? Distortion?
31
?
32
The Pool Ambiguous? Paradoxical? Fictitious? Distortion?
33
Penrose Trident Ambiguous? Paradoxical? Fictitious? Distortion?
34
?
35
?
36
? Keep staring at the blue dot in the middle of the picture.
37
? In which direction is the horse facing?
38
Morinaga’s paradox
39
In reality both judgments, of vertical alignment and of the horizontal gaps, are illusions. The tips of the arrows are perfectly aligned vertically, and the horizontal gaps between the three sets of arrowheads are all exactly the same. (This is a version of the Muller-Lyer illusion!)
40
Variation with Ambihelical Hexnut Ambiguous? Paradoxical? Fictitious? Distortion?
41
The Terrace Ambiguous? Paradoxical? Fictitious? Distortion?
42
?
43
Ehrenstein Illusion Ambiguous? Paradoxical? Fictitious? Distortion?
44
?
45
?
46
Zöllner illusion Ambiguous? Paradoxical? Fictitious? Distortion?
47
Poggendorff Illusion Ambiguous? Paradoxical? Fictitious? Distortion?
48
Apparent Motion Object disappears and reappears somewhere else Visual system “interpolates” motion in between
49
Is this a spiral ? Ambiguous? Paradoxical? Fictitious? Distortion?
50
Frazer Spiral
51
? Look just above or below the circle. Keep looking forward and move your head to the left and the right.
52
? Are you looking into a tiled room? Or at a cube ?
53
How the brain allows us to see a stable world The brain allows us to see a stable consistent world using: Visual constancies and Depth cues
54
What shape is this door?
55
What colour are the squares?
56
Who is the taller?
57
Retinal images Our perception of objects is far more constant or stable than the images that reaches our eye. The images coming into our eyes change with the movement of: the eyes, the head, position, the changing light. What would happen if we relied only on retinal images for visual perception?
58
Keeping the world constant If we relied only on retinal images for visual perception we would always be: conscious of people growing physically bigger when they came closer, objects changing their shapes whenever we moved, and colours changing with every shift in lighting conditions
59
Visual constancies Visual constancy is the ability to recognize that an object remains the same even when it produces different images on the retina (it looks different) There are 3 types of visual constancy 1. Size 2. Shape 3. Colour
60
Who is the taller?
61
Size constancy Under normal conditions we know that the tiny people, cars, and buildings we see at a great distance are not miniatures, but appear small because they are far away – this is size constancy.
62
What shape is this door?
63
Shape constancy a door appears to change shape as it is opened. Shape constancy ensures that we are not typically conscious of this
64
What colour are the squares?
65
Colour constancy Colour constancy makes sure we don't realise/notice that squares A and B have the same colour.
66
Size constancy example (a)
67
Size constancy example (b)
68
Size constancy example (c)
69
Size constancy example (d)
70
Size constancy example (e)
71
The Role of Expectation - 1 Expectation can influence perception. Showing participants an ambiguous figure '13' set in the context of letters or numbers can affect what is “seen”
72
Continued
73
The Role of Expectation - 2
74
The need to perceive distance/depth Our survival as individuals and as a species depends on the ability to judge distance and depth We need to locate objects in space and perceive depth since this ability is essential for almost all activities, e.g. navigating/avoiding objects jumping catching/throwing reaching/grasping size judgements and recognition
75
The problem of distance/depth The distance of an object (or its depth in the visual field) depends on 3-dimensional perception BUT the image that falls on our retina is 2-dimensional!!
76
How do we perceive distance/depth? Therefore, we need to be able to translate a two dimensional retinal image into a three dimensional picture This is something that humans – and many other species as well - can do with remarkable accuracy. SO, then, how do we perceive DEPTH and DISTANCE?
77
Visual Cues – Binocular/Monocular We use many cues to determine depth and distance, which fall into two types; some of the cues depend on us having two eyes and are known as binocular cues, others depend on cues from one eye only and are known as monocular cues.
78
Binocular vision Uses BINOCULAR depth cues – i.e. inform- ation from BOTH eyes. Demonstration:- Hold two pencils at arm’s length away from your body. Close one eye. Try to bring the pencils together so that the points touch each other. Repeat this with the other eye closed and then with both eyes open. Is it easier to do this with one eye open or with both eyes open? Why?
79
Binocular depth cues Depth cues that depend on two eyes working together 1. Convergence Occurs when the eyes turn inward to focus on nearby objects – the closer the object, the greater the convergence 2. Retinal disparity (or Binocular disparity) Difference between the two retinal images formed by the eyes’ slightly different views of the objects focused on
80
Binocular cues - Convergence Our brain uses information from the eye muscles in order to provide us with infor- mation about how far away the object is. NB. Convergence only works for objects which are closer than about 10 feet away After that the difference in convergence is too slight to provide useful data
81
Binocular cues - Retinal Disparity Since our eyes are about 2 inches apart, each one receives a slightly different image of any object we are observing. The nearer the object, the greater the retinal disparity. The brain is able to use the amount of retinal disparity as an indication of depth and distance
82
Monocular depth cues Depth cues that can be perceived using only one eye
83
1 Interposition When one object partly blocks your view of another, you perceive the partially blocked object as farther away
84
2 Linear perspective Parallel lines that are known to be the same distance apart appear to grow closer together, or converge, as they recede into the distance
85
3. Relative size Larger objects are perceived as being closer to the viewer, and smaller objects as being farther away
86
4 Height in the visual field
87
5. Texture gradient Near objects appear to have sharply defined textures, while similar objects appear fuzzier as they recede into the distance
88
An Example of Texture Gradients Creating Depth
89
6. Atmospheric (aerial) perspective Objects in the distance have a bluish tint and appear more blurred than objects close at hand (caused by the density of water vapour in the atmosphere which makes colours appear “cooler”)
90
7 Motion parallax When you ride in a moving vehicle and look out the side window, the objects you see outside appear to be moving in the opposite direction Objects seem to be moving at different speeds – those closest to you appear to be moving faster than those in the distance
91
Art and Illusion Many artists have used visual illusions in their work Most famous:- Dali Escher Ocampo
92
Dali - 1
93
Dali - 2 Paranoiac Visage - The Postcard Transformed
94
Dali - 3 Mysterious Mouth Appearing in the Back of My Nurse
95
Dali - 4 Transformation of 'Antiques' Magazine Cover into the Apparition of a Face
96
Dali - 5
97
Escher - 1
98
Escher - 2
99
Escher - 3
100
The General’s Family - Ocampo There are nine faces in this picture
101
Mona Lisa’s chair - Ocampo
102
Perceptual organisation When we perceive the world we impose an order and structure which cannot be found in any of its particular units or components. The processes by which we impose order or structure on our sensations, making them into coherent whole, are referred to as processes of perceptual organisation.
103
Laws of organisation The Gestalt psychologists put forward a set of laws of organisation: i.e. general rules by which we perceive shapes and forms and which the Gestalists believe are innate. The laws of organisation are not hard and fast rules which always apply, neither are they explanations; but they do provide a reasonably accurate description of how we usually perceive the world.
104
Perception Theories - Gestalt Key Points:- We perceive objects as well-organized patterns rather than separate components. “The whole is greater than the sum of it’s parts.” Based on the concept of “grouping”.
105
1 Figure/Ground We perceive things in terms of figure and (back)ground. e.g. If you look around the room, you may see books on a table, or a clock on the mantlepiece. All of these are figures which have a definite location in space. These figures stand out against the background which has no definite shape and seems to continue behind the figures.
106
Rubin vase - 1 Occasionally (usually in specially designed pictures) we have difficulty in separating figure from ground, and experience an illusion, e.g. the Rubin vase
107
Rubin vase - 2 This illusion can also be affected by the principle of smallness: i.e smaller areas tend to be seen as figures against a larger background.
108
Figure/Background Figure – seen as the foreground Ground – seen as the background Contours – “belong” to the figure
109
Camouflage Camouflage techniques rely on making the figure and ground indistinguishable.
110
The Law of Proximity When we see things that are close to- gether we tend to see them as a group rather than as separate items. e.g. we tend to three pairs of lines and a single line rather than seven – separate - lines.
111
The Law of Closure - 1 We tend to see figures as a complete whole even though there are gaps. E.g. we see the shape below as a circle even though it is not joined up.
112
The Law of Closure - 2
113
The Law of Similarity - 1 Things that are in some way alike, in terms of shape, colour or size, are usually per- ceived as belonging together as part of a pattern.
114
The Law of Similarity - 2 This image is seen as consisting of noughts and crosses grouped together in horizontal lines, not as noughts and crosses alternating with each other in vertical columns XXXXX OOOOO XXXXX OOOOO
115
The Law of Continuity Lines and patterns tend to be continued in space. In this figure, you are more likely to per- ceive a single line partially covered by a circle rather than a circle and two sep- arate lines.
116
The Law of Common Fate When single elements are moving in the same direction at the same speed they are seen as a whole. e.g. a flock of birds flying overhead is seen as one element, not as a group of individual birds.
117
‘Good form' and ‘Belonging' These ‘laws’ can essentially be reduced to two: 1. the law of Pragnanz or 'good form‘, and 2.the law of 'belonging'. The law of Pragnanz refers to the fact that we tend to see things as a coherent whole, thus we close gaps and make objects more symmetrical. The law of belonging specifies the ways in which we group certain elements within a stimulus pattern.
118
Summary - 1 Figure-ground Organization depends on what we see as figure (object) and what we perceive a ground (context). Similarity Objects that have similar characteristics are perceived as unit. Proximity Objects close together in space or time perceived as belonging together.
119
Summary - 2 Continuity We tend to perceive figures or objects as belonging together if they appear to form a continuous pattern. Closure We perceive figures with gaps in them to be complete
120
Summary - 3 Gestalt laws of Grouping organize the visual scene into units The Law of Goodness of Figure creates the simplest most meaningful pattern Figure/Ground relationships define important parts of the scene
121
Gregory’s theory of visual illusions Gregory (1963) suggests that some visual illusions can be explained in terms of misapplied perceptual constancy. Within the Ponzo illusion there are cues which indicate that this represents a three dimensional scene
122
Ponzo Illusion The lines at the side could be parallel lines disappearing into the distance. One horizontal line above another could represent different heights in the visual field. Both of these are depth cues.
123
Ponzo Illusion These two cues, then, suggest that the top line is further in the distance than the line beneath it. Since the retinal image of both is the same size, the principal of size constancy encourages us to perceive the top line as being longer than the bottom one.
124
Muller-Lyer illusion Gregory has suggested that, in the absence of other cues, the figure with the fins diverging represents an inside corner and the one with the fins converging represents an outside corner
125
Muller-Lyer illusion Past experience indicates that a vertical line between diverging arrowheads is farther away than the ceiling and floor lines represented by the arrowheads. Conversely, converging arrowheads suggest that the vertical line is closer than the lines receding from it.
126
Muller-Lyer illusion Since the two lines appear to be at different distances but are the same retinal size, the one which appears to be farther away is perceived as longer.
127
Evaluation Gregory's theory of misapplied constancy can easily be applied to the Ponzo illusion However, it is more problematic when applied to the Muller-Lyer illusion.
128
Evidence in favour - 1
129
Evidence in favour - 2 This illusion is barely experienced by people who have lived in an environment which has given them little experience of linear perspective, e.g. people such as the congenitally blind who have restored sight, and Zulus whose environment consists of rounded buildings and who have little experience of objects with straight edges and right-angled corners.
130
Evidence against - 1 There are several versions of the Muller- Lyer illusion for which this theory cannot account: 1 The illusion still persists if the lines are horizontal, yet this is not the way we perceive the edges of buildings.
131
Evidence against - 2 2. If the lines are removed altogether and only the fins left in place, the distance between the fins with the points facing inwards appears greater than the distance between the fins with the points facing outwards.
132
Evidence against - 3 The illusion remains very strong when the fins are replaced by circles, squares or several other shapes, These obviously cannot be accounted for by architectural features providing misleading depth cues.
133
Day's theory - 1 Day (1972) has put forward a more general theory which accounts for some types of illusions. We simultaneously use many cues, such as texture gradient, relative size and interposition, to judge distance and depth. According to Day, when some of these cues are so strong that they override others, they can mislead us so that our judgment of depth, distance and size become distorted.
134
Day's theory - 2 Examples of a visual illusion explained by Day's theory:
135
Other theories: Visual Coding Some visual illusions may be caused by coding in the visual system This theory focuses on the way in which certain components of the stimulus are (pre-)coded into the visual system. For example, acute angles are usually judged as being less acute than they really are.
136
Visual Coding The illusion shown below involves the perception of acute angles, this could explain the apparent distortion of the lines.
Similar presentations
© 2025 SlidePlayer.com. Inc.
All rights reserved.