Download presentation
Presentation is loading. Please wait.
1
Chapter 51 Animal Behavior
2
Overview: The How and Why of Animal Activity
Fiddler crabs feed with their small claw and wave their large claw Why do male fiddler crabs engage in claw waving behavior? Claw waving is used to repel other males and to attract females © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
3
Figure 51.1 Figure 51.1 What prompts a fiddler crab to wave its giant claw? 3
4
Behavior is subject to natural selection
A behavior is the nervous system’s response to a stimulus and is carried out by the muscular or the hormonal system Behavior is subject to natural selection © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
5
Video: Albatross Courtship Ritual
© 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
6
Video: Blue-footed Boobies Courtship Ritual
© 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
7
Video: Giraffe Courtship Ritual
© 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
8
Concept 51.1: Discrete sensory inputs can stimulate both simple and complex behaviors
Niko Tinbergen identified four questions that should be asked about animal behavior What stimulus elicits the behavior, and what physiological mechanisms mediate the response? How does the animal’s experience during growth and development influence the response? © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
9
3. How does the behavior aid survival and reproduction?
What is the behavior’s evolutionary history? Behavioral ecology is the study of the ecological and evolutionary basis for animal behavior © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
10
Behavioral ecology integrates proximate and ultimate explanations for animal behavior
Proximate causation addresses “how” a behavior occurs or is modified, including Tinbergen’s questions 1 and 2 Ultimate causation addresses “why” a behavior occurs in the context of natural selection, including Tinbergen’s questions 3 and 4 © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
11
Fixed Action Patterns A fixed action pattern is a sequence of unlearned, innate behaviors that is unchangeable Once initiated, it is usually carried to completion A fixed action pattern is triggered by an external cue known as a sign stimulus © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
12
Tinbergen observed male stickleback fish responding to a passing red truck
In male stickleback fish, the stimulus for attack behavior is the red underside of an intruder When presented with unrealistic models, the attack behavior occurs as long as some red is present © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
13
Figure 51.2 (a) Figure 51.2 Sign stimuli in a classic fixed action pattern. (b) 13
14
Migration Environmental cues can trigger movement in a particular direction Migration is a regular, long-distance change in location Animals can orient themselves using The position of the sun and their circadian clock, an internal 24-hour clock that is an integral part of their nervous system The position of the North Star The Earth’s magnetic field © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
15
Figure 51.3 Figure 51.3 Migration. 15
16
Behavioral Rhythms Some animal behavior is affected by the animal’s circadian rhythm, a daily cycle of rest and activity Behaviors such as migration and reproduction are linked to changing seasons, or a circannual rhythm Daylight and darkness are common seasonal cues Some behaviors are linked to lunar cycles, which affect tidal movements © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
17
Animal Signals and Communication
In behavioral ecology, a signal is a behavior that causes a change in another animal’s behavior Communication is the transmission and reception of signals © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
18
Forms of Animal Communication
Animals communicate using visual, chemical, tactile, and auditory signals Fruit fly courtship follows a three step stimulus-response chain © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
19
Chemical communication: he smells a female’s chemicals in the air
1. A male identifies a female of the same species and orients toward her Chemical communication: he smells a female’s chemicals in the air Visual communication: he sees the female and orients his body toward hers © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
20
2. The male alerts the female to his presence
Tactile communication: he taps the female with a foreleg Chemical communication: he chemically confirms the female’s identity © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
21
Auditory communication: he extends and vibrates his wing
3. The male produces a courtship song to inform the female of his species Auditory communication: he extends and vibrates his wing If all three steps are successful, the female will allow the male to copulate © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
22
(a) Orienting (b) Tapping (c) “Singing” Figure 51.4
Figure 51.4 Courtship behavior of the fruit fly. (a) Orienting (b) Tapping (c) “Singing” 22
23
Honeybees show complex communication with symbolic language
A bee returning from the field performs a dance to communicate information about the distance and direction of a food source © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
24
Figure 51.5 Honeybee dance language.
(a) Worker bees (b) Round dance (food near) Waggle dance (food distant) (c) A 30° Figure 51.5 Honeybee dance language. C B Beehive 30° Location A Location B Location C 24
25
Figure 51.5a Figure 51.5 Honeybee dance language. (a) Worker bees 25
26
Round dance (food near)
Figure 51.5b Figure 51.5 Honeybee dance language. (b) Round dance (food near) 26
27
Waggle dance (food distant)
Figure 51.5c (c) Waggle dance (food distant) A 30° C B Beehive 30° Figure 51.5 Honeybee dance language. Location A Location B Location C 27
28
Pheromones Many animals that communicate through odors emit chemical substances called pheromones © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
29
For example, A female moth can attract a male moth several kilometers distant A honeybee queen produces a pheromone that affects the development and behavior of female workers and male drones When a minnow or catfish is injured, an alarm substance in the fish’s skin disperses in the water, inducing a fright response among fish in the area © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
30
Minnows before alarm (a) Minnows after alarm (b) Figure 51.6
Figure 51.6 Minnows responding to the presence of an alarm substance. 30
31
Pheromones can be effective at very low concentrations
Nocturnal animals, such as most terrestrial mammals, depend on olfactory and auditory communication Diurnal animals, such as humans and most birds, use visual and auditory communication © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
32
Concept 51.2: Learning establishes specific links between experience and behavior
Innate behavior is developmentally fixed and does not vary among individuals © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
33
Experience and Behavior
Cross-fostering studies help behavioral ecologists to identify the contribution of environment to an animal’s behavior A cross-fostering study places the young from one species in the care of adults from another species © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
34
Studies of California mice and white-footed mice have uncovered an influence of social environment on aggressive and parental behaviors Cross-fostered mice developed some behaviors that were consistent with their foster parents © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
35
Table 51.1 Table 51.1 Influence of Cross-Fostering on Male Mice 35
36
In humans, twin studies allow researchers to compare the relative influences of genetics and environment on behavior © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
37
Learning Learning is the modification of behavior based on specific experiences © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
38
Imprinting Imprinting is a behavior that includes learning and innate components and is generally irreversible It is distinguished from other learning by a sensitive period A sensitive period is a limited developmental phase that is the only time when certain behaviors can be learned © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
39
An example of imprinting is young geese following their mother
Konrad Lorenz showed that when baby geese spent the first few hours of their life with him, they imprinted on him as their parent The imprint stimulus in greylag geese is a nearby object that is moving away from the young geese © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
40
Video: Ducklings © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
41
(a) Konrad Lorenz and geese
Figure 51.7 (a) Konrad Lorenz and geese Figure 51.7 Imprinting. (b) Pilot and cranes 41
42
(a) Konrad Lorenz and geese
Figure 51.7a Figure 51.7 Imprinting. (a) Konrad Lorenz and geese 42
43
Conservation biologists have taken advantage of imprinting in programs to save the whooping crane from extinction Young whooping cranes can imprint on humans in “crane suits” who then lead crane migrations using ultralight aircraft © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
44
Figure 51.7b Figure 51.7 Imprinting. (b) Pilot and cranes 44
45
Spatial Learning and Cognitive Maps
Spatial learning is a more complex modification of behavior based on experience with the spatial structure of the environment Niko Tinbergen showed how digger wasps use landmarks to find nest entrances © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
46
Video: Bee Pollinating
© 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
47
EXPERIMENT Nest Pinecone RESULTS Nest No nest Figure 51.8
Figure 51.8 Inquiry: Does a digger wasp use landmarks to find her nest? Nest No nest 47
48
A cognitive map is an internal representation of spatial relationships between objects in an animal’s surroundings For example, Clark’s nutcrackers can find food hidden in caches located halfway between particular landmarks © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
49
Associative Learning In associative learning, animals associate one feature of their environment with another For example, a white-footed mouse will avoid eating caterpillars with specific colors after a bad experience with a distasteful monarch butterfly caterpillar © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
50
Classical conditioning is a type of associative learning in which an arbitrary stimulus is associated with a reward or punishment For example, a dog that repeatedly hears a bell before being fed will salivate in anticipation at the bell’s sound © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
51
It is also called trial-and-error learning
Operant conditioning is a type of associative learning in which an animal learns to associate one of its behaviors with a reward or punishment It is also called trial-and-error learning For example, a rat that is fed after pushing a lever will learn to push the lever in order to receive food For example, a predator may learn to avoid a specific type of prey associated with a painful experience © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
52
Figure 51.9 Figure 51.9 Associative learning. 52
53
Figure 51.9a Figure 51.9 Associative learning. 53
54
Figure 51.9b Figure 51.9 Associative learning. 54
55
Figure 51.9c Figure 51.9 Associative learning. 55
56
Cognition and Problem Solving
Cognition is a process of knowing that may include awareness, reasoning, recollection, and judgment For example, honeybees can distinguish “same” from “different” © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
57
Decision chamber Food Stimulus Lid Entrance (a) Color maze
Figure 51.10 Decision chamber Food Stimulus Lid Figure A maze test of abstract thinking by honeybees. Entrance (a) Color maze (b) Pattern maze 57
58
Problem solving is the process of devising a strategy to overcome an obstacle
For example, chimpanzees can stack boxes in order to reach suspended food For example, ravens obtained food suspended from a branch by a string by pulling up the string © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
59
Video: Chimp Cracking Nut
© 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
60
Development of Learned Behaviors
Development of some behaviors occurs in distinct stages For example a white-crowned sparrow memorizes the song of its species during an early sensitive period The bird then learns to sing the song during a second learning phase © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
61
Social Learning Social learning is learning through the observation of others and forms the roots of culture For example, young chimpanzees learn to crack palm nuts with stones by copying older chimpanzees For example, vervet monkeys give and respond to distinct alarm calls for different predators © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
62
Figure 51.11 Figure A young chimpanzee learning to crack oil palm nuts by observing an experienced elder. 62
63
Figure 51.12 Figure Vervet monkeys learning correct use of alarm calls. 63
64
Figure 51.12a Figure Vervet monkeys learning correct use of alarm calls. 64
65
Figure 51.12b Figure Vervet monkeys learning correct use of alarm calls. 65
66
Culture can alter behavior and influence the fitness of individuals
Culture is a system of information transfer through observation or teaching that influences behavior of individuals in a population Culture can alter behavior and influence the fitness of individuals © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
67
Concept 51.3: Selection for individual survival and reproductive success can explain most behaviors
Behavior enhances survival and reproductive success in a population © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
68
Foraging Behavior Natural selection refines behaviors that enhance the efficiency of feeding Foraging, or food-obtaining behavior, includes recognizing, searching for, capturing, and eating food items © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
69
Evolution of Foraging Behavior
In Drosophila melanogaster, variation in a gene dictates foraging behavior in the larvae Larvae with one allele travel farther while foraging than larvae with the other allele Larvae in high-density populations benefit from foraging farther for food, while larvae in low-density populations benefit from short-distance foraging © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
70
Natural selection favors different alleles depending on the density of the population
Under laboratory conditions, evolutionary changes in the frequency of these two alleles were observed over several generations © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
71
Low population density 6 High population density
Figure 51.13 7 Low population density 6 High population density 5 4 Mean path length (cm) 3 2 Figure Evolution of foraging behavior by laboratory populations of Drosophila melanogaster. 1 R1 R2 R3 K1 K2 K3 D. melanogaster lineages 71
72
Optimal Foraging Model
Optimal foraging model views foraging behavior as a compromise between benefits of nutrition and costs of obtaining food The costs of obtaining food include energy expenditure and the risk of being eaten while foraging Natural selection should favor foraging behavior that minimizes the costs and maximizes the benefits © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
73
Optimal foraging behavior is demonstrated by the Northwestern crow
A crow will drop a whelk (a mollusc) from a height to break its shell and feed on the soft parts The crow faces a trade-off between the height from which it drops the whelk and the number of times it must drop the whelk © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
74
The average flight height for crows is 5.23 m
Researchers determined experimentally that the total flight height (which reflects total energy expenditure) was minimized at a drop height of 5 m The average flight height for crows is 5.23 m © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
75
Average number of drops
Figure 51.14 125 60 50 100 40 Average number of drops Average number of drops 30 75 Total flight height Total flight height (number of drops drop height in m) 20 Drop height preferred by crows 5.23 m Figure Energy costs and benefits in foraging behavior. 50 10 25 2 3 5 7 15 Drop height (m) 75
76
Balancing Risk and Reward
Risk of predation affects foraging behavior For example, mule deer are more likely to feed in open forested areas where they are less likely to be killed by mountain lions © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
77
Mating Behavior and Mate Choice
Mating behavior includes seeking or attracting mates, choosing among potential mates, competing for mates, and caring for offspring Mating relationships define a number of distinct mating systems © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
78
Mating Systems and Sexual Dimorphism
The mating relationship between males and females varies greatly from species to species In many species, mating is promiscuous, with no strong pair-bonds or lasting relationships In monogamous relationships, one male mates with one female Males and females with monogamous mating systems have similar external morphologies © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
79
Figure 51.15 (a) Monogamous species (b) Polygynous species Figure Relationship between mating system and male and female forms. (c) Polyandrous species 79
80
(a) Monogamous species
Figure 51.15a Figure Relationship between mating system and male and female forms. (a) Monogamous species 80
81
Polygamous relationships can be either polygynous or polyandrous
In polygamous relationships, an individual of one sex mates with several individuals of the other sex Species with polygamous mating systems are usually sexually dimorphic: males and females have different external morphologies Polygamous relationships can be either polygynous or polyandrous © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
82
In polygyny, one male mates with many females
The males are usually more showy and larger than the females © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
83
(b) Polygynous species
Figure 51.15b Figure Relationship between mating system and male and female forms. (b) Polygynous species 83
84
In polyandry, one female mates with many males
The females are often more showy than the males © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
85
(c) Polyandrous species
Figure 51.15c Figure Relationship between mating system and male and female forms. (c) Polyandrous species 85
86
Mating Systems and Parental Care
Needs of the young are an important factor constraining evolution of mating systems © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
87
Consider bird species where chicks need a continuous supply of food
A male maximizes his reproductive success by staying with his mate and caring for his chicks (monogamy) Consider bird species where chicks are soon able to feed and care for themselves A male maximizes his reproductive success by seeking additional mates (polygyny) © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
88
Certainty of paternity influences parental care and mating behavior
Females can be certain that eggs laid or young born contain her genes; however, paternal certainty depends on mating behavior Paternal certainty is relatively low in species with internal fertilization because mating and birth are separated over time © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
89
Certainty of paternity is much higher when egg laying and mating occur together, as in external fertilization In species with external fertilization, parental care is at least as likely to be by males as by females © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
90
Figure 51.16 Figure Paternal care by a male jawfish. 90
91
Sexual Selection and Mate Choice
Sexual dimorphism results from sexual selection, a form of natural selection In intersexual selection, members of one sex choose mates on the basis of certain traits Intrasexual selection involves competition between members of the same sex for mates © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
92
Mate Choice by Females Female choice is a type of intersexual competition Females can drive sexual selection by choosing males with specific behaviors or features of anatomy For example, female stalk-eyed flies choose males with relatively long eyestalks Ornaments, such as long eyestalks, often correlate with health and vitality © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
93
Figure 51.17 Figure Male stalk-eyed flies. 93
94
Another example of mate choice by females occurs in zebra finches
Female chicks who imprint on ornamented fathers are more likely to select ornamented mates Experiments suggest that mate choice by female zebra finches has played a key role in the evolution of ornamentation in male zebra finches © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
95
Figure 51.18 Figure Appearance of zebra finches in nature. 95
96
Experimental Groups of Parental Pairs Control Group
Figure 51.19 Experimental Groups of Parental Pairs Control Group Both parents ornamented Males ornamented Females ornamented Parents not ornamented Offspring Offspring Figure Sexual selection influenced by imprinting. Mate preference of female offspring: ornamented male Mate preference of female offspring: none 96
97
Mate-choice copying is a behavior in which individuals copy the mate choice of others
For example, in an experiment with guppies, the choice of female models influenced the choice of other females © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
98
Male guppies with varying degrees of coloration
Figure 51.20 Control Sample Male guppies with varying degrees of coloration Female guppies prefer males with more orange coloration. Experimental Sample Figure Mate choice copying by female guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Female model in mock courtship with less orange male Female guppies prefer males that are associated with another female. 98
99
Male guppies with varying degrees of coloration
Figure 51.20a Control Sample Male guppies with varying degrees of coloration Female guppies prefer males with more orange coloration. Figure Mate choice copying by female guppies (Poecilia reticulata). 99
100
Female model in mock courtship with less orange male
Figure 51.20b Experimental Sample Female model in mock courtship with less orange male Figure Mate choice copying by female guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Female guppies prefer males that are associated with another female. 100
101
Male Competition for Mates
Male competition for mates is a source of intrasexual selection that can reduce variation among males Such competition may involve agonistic behavior, an often ritualized contest that determines which competitor gains access to a resource © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
102
Figure 51.21 Figure Agonistic interaction. 102
103
Video: Chimp Agonistic Behavior
© 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
104
Video: Snake Ritual Wrestling
© 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
105
Video: Wolves Agonistic Behavior
© 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
106
Applying Game Theory In some species, sexual selection has driven the evolution of alternative mating behavior and morphology in males The fitness of a particular phenotype (behavior or morphology) depends on the phenotypes of other individuals in the population Game theory evaluates alternative strategies where the outcome depends on each individual’s strategy and the strategy of other individuals © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
107
Each color is associated with a specific strategy for obtaining mates
For example, each side-blotched lizard has a blue, orange, or yellow throat Each color is associated with a specific strategy for obtaining mates Orange-throat males are the most aggressive and defend large territories Blue-throats defend small territories Yellow-throats are nonterritorial, mimic females, and use “sneaky” strategies to mate © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
108
Figure 51.22 Figure Male polymorphism in the side-blotched lizard (Uta stansburiana). 108
109
Like rock-paper-scissors, each strategy will outcompete one strategy but be outcompeted by the other strategy The success of each strategy depends on the frequency of all of the strategies; this drives frequency-dependent selection © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
110
Concept 51.4: Inclusive fitness can account for the evolution of behavior, including altruism
Animal behavior is governed by complex interactions between genetic and environmental factors Selfless behavior can be explained by inclusive fitness © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
111
Genetic Basis of Behavior
A master regulatory gene can control many behaviors For example, a single gene controls many behaviors of the male fruit fly courtship ritual Multiple independent genes can contribute to a single behavior For example, in green lacewings, the courtship song is unique to each species; multiple independent genes govern different components of the courtship song © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
112
F1 hybrids, typical phenotype: Chrysoperla plorabunda parent:
Figure 51.23 EXPERIMENT RESULTS SOUND RECORDINGS F1 hybrids, typical phenotype: Chrysoperla plorabunda parent: Volley period Volley period Standard repeating unit Vibration volleys Standard repeating unit crossed with Chrysoperla johnsoni parent: Volley period Figure Inquiry: Are the songs of green lacewing species under the control of multiple genes? Standard repeating unit 112
113
Figure 51.23a Figure Inquiry: Are the songs of green lacewing species under the control of multiple genes? 113
114
Differences at a single locus can sometimes have a large effect on behavior
For example, male prairie voles pair-bond with their mates, while male meadow voles do not The level of a specific receptor for a neurotransmitter determines which behavioral pattern develops © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
115
Figure 51.24 Figure A pair of prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) huddling. 115
116
Genetic Variation and the Evolution of Behavior
When behavioral variation within a species corresponds to environmental variation, it may be evidence of past evolution © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
117
Case Study: Variation in Prey Selection
The natural diet of western garter snakes varies by population Coastal populations feed mostly on banana slugs, while inland populations rarely eat banana slugs Studies have shown that the differences in diet are genetic The two populations differ in their ability to detect and respond to specific odor molecules produced by the banana slugs © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
118
Figure 51.25 Figure Western garter snake from a coastal habitat eating a banana slug. 118
119
Case Study: Variation in Migratory Patterns
Most blackcaps (birds) that breed in Germany winter in Africa, but some winter in Britain Under laboratory conditions, each migratory population exhibits different migratory behaviors The migratory behaviors are regulated by genetics © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
120
Adults from Britain and offspring of British
Figure 51.26 EXPERIMENT Scratch marks RESULTS Figure Inquiry: Are differences in migratory orientation within a species genetically determined? W N E S Adults from Britain and offspring of British adults BRITAIN GERMANY W N E S Young from SW Germany 120
121
EXPERIMENT Scratch marks Figure 51.26a
Figure Inquiry: Are differences in migratory orientation within a species genetically determined? 121
122
RESULTS N BRITAIN W E GERMANY S N W E S
Figure 51.26b RESULTS N W S E Adults from Britain and offspring of British adults BRITAIN GERMANY N W S E Figure Inquiry: Are differences in migratory orientation within a species genetically determined? Young from SW Germany 122
123
Altruism Natural selection favors behavior that maximizes an individual’s survival and reproduction These behaviors are often selfish On occasion, some animals behave in ways that reduce their individual fitness but increase the fitness of others This kind of behavior is called altruism, or selflessness © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
124
For example, under threat from a predator, an individual Belding’s ground squirrel will make an alarm call to warn others, even though calling increases the chances that the caller is killed For example, in naked mole rat populations, nonreproductive individuals may sacrifice their lives protecting their reproductive queen and kings from predators © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
125
Figure 51.27 Figure Naked mole rats, a species of colonial mammal that exhibits altruistic behavior. 125
126
Inclusive Fitness Altruism can be explained by inclusive fitness
Inclusive fitness is the total effect an individual has on proliferating its genes by producing offspring and helping close relatives produce offspring © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
127
Hamilton’s Rule and Kin Selection
William Hamilton proposed a quantitative measure for predicting when natural selection would favor altruistic acts among related individuals Three key variables in an altruistic act Benefit to the recipient (B) Cost to the altruistic (C) Coefficient of relatedness (the fraction of genes that, on average, are shared; r) © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
128
Parent A Parent B 1/2 (0.5) probability 1/2 (0.5) probability
Figure 51.28 Parent A Parent B OR Figure The coefficient of relatedness between siblings. 1/2 (0.5) probability 1/2 (0.5) probability Sibling 1 Sibling 2 128
129
Natural selection favors altruism when rB > C
This inequality is called Hamilton’s rule Hamilton’s rule is illustrated with the following example of a girl who risks her life to save her brother © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
130
If the sister saves her brother rB ( 1) C ( 0.5)
Assume the average individual has two children. As a result of the sister’s action The brother can now father two children, so B 2 The sister has a 25% chance of dying and not being able to have two children, so C 0.25 2 0.5 The brother and sister share half their genes on average, so r 0.5 If the sister saves her brother rB ( 1) C ( 0.5) © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
131
In a group, most of the females are closely related to each other
Kin selection is the natural selection that favors this kind of altruistic behavior by enhancing reproductive success of relatives An example of kin selection and altruism is the warning behavior in Belding’s ground squirrels In a group, most of the females are closely related to each other Most alarm calls are given by females who are likely aiding close relatives © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
132
Mean distance (m) moved from birthplace
Figure 51.29 300 Male Mean distance (m) moved from birthplace 200 100 Figure Kin selection and altruism in Belding’s ground squirrels. Female 1 2 3 4 12 13 14 15 25 26 Age (months) 132
133
Figure 51.29a Figure Kin selection and altruism in Belding’s ground squirrels. 133
134
Naked mole rats living within a colony are closely related
Nonreproductive individuals increase their inclusive fitness by helping the reproductive queen and kings (their close relatives) to pass their genes to the next generation © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
135
Reciprocal Altruism Altruistic behavior toward unrelated individuals can be adaptive if the aided individual returns the favor in the future This type of altruism is called reciprocal altruism © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
136
Reciprocal altruism is limited to species with stable social groups where individuals meet repeatedly, and cheaters (who don’t reciprocate) are punished Reciprocal altruism has been used to explain altruism between unrelated individuals in humans © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
137
In game theory, a tit-for-tat strategy has the following rules:
Individuals always cooperate on first encounter An individual treats another the same way it was treated the last time they met That is, individuals will always cooperate, unless their opponent cheated them the last time they met © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
138
Tit-for-tat strategy explains how reciprocal altruism could have evolved
Individuals who engage in a tit-for-tat strategy have a higher fitness than individuals who are always selfish © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
139
Evolution and Human Culture
No other species comes close to matching the social learning and cultural transmission that occur among humans Human culture is related to evolutionary theory in the distinct discipline of sociobiology Human behavior, like that of other species, results from interaction between genes and environment © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
140
However, our social and cultural institutions may provide the only feature in which there is no continuum between humans and other animals © 2011 Pearson Education, Inc.
141
Learning and problem solving
Figure 51.UN01 Imprinting Learning and problem solving Spatial learning Cognition Figure 51.UN01 Summary figure, Concept 51.2 Associative learning Social learning 141
142
Figure 51.UN02 Figure 51.UN02 Appendix A: answer to Test Your Understanding, question 7 142
Similar presentations
© 2025 SlidePlayer.com. Inc.
All rights reserved.