Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Albert Gatt Corpora and Statistical Methods. In this lecture Corpora and Statistical Methods We have considered distributions of words and lexical variation.

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Albert Gatt Corpora and Statistical Methods. In this lecture Corpora and Statistical Methods We have considered distributions of words and lexical variation."— Presentation transcript:

1 Albert Gatt Corpora and Statistical Methods

2 In this lecture Corpora and Statistical Methods We have considered distributions of words and lexical variation in corpora. Today we consider collocations: definition and characteristics measures of collocational strength experiments on corpora hypothesis testing

3 Collocations: Definition and characteristics Part 1

4 A motivating example Corpora and Statistical Methods Consider phrases such as: strong tea? powerful tea strong support? powerful support powerful drug? strong drug Traditional semantic theories have difficulty accounting for these patterns. strong and powerful seem near-synonyms do we claim they have different senses? what is the crucial difference?

5 The empiricist view of meaning Corpora and Statistical Methods Firth’s view (1957): “You shall know a word by the company it keeps” This is a contextual view of meaning, akin to that espoused by Wittgenstein (1953). In the Firthian tradition, attention is paid to patterns that crop up with regularity in language. Contrast symbolic/rationalist approaches, emphasising polysemy, componential analysis, etc. Statistical work on collocations tends to follow this tradition.

6 Defining collocations Corpora and Statistical Methods “Collocations … are statements of the habitual or customary places of [a] word.” (Firth 1957) Characteristics/Expectations: regular/frequently attested; occur within a narrow window (span of few words); not fully compositional; non-substitutable; non-modifiable display category restrictions

7 Frequency and regularity Corpora and Statistical Methods We know that language is regular (non-random) and rule- based. this aspect is emphasised by rationalist approaches to grammar We also need to acknowledge that frequency of usage is an important factor in language development. why do big and large collocate differently with different nouns?

8 Regularity/frequency Corpora and Statistical Methods f(strong tea) > f(powerful tea) f(credit card) > f(credit bankruptcy) f(white wine) > f(yellow wine) (even though white wine is actually yellowish)

9 Narrow window (textual proximity) Corpora and Statistical Methods Usually, we specify an n-gram window within which to analyse collocations: bigram: credit card, credit crunch trigram: credit card fraud, credit card expiry … The idea is to look at co-occurrence of words within a specific n-gram window We can also count n-grams with intervening words: federal (.*) subsidy matches: federal subsidy, federal farm subsidy, federal manufacturing subsidy…

10 Textual proximity (continued) Corpora and Statistical Methods Usually collocates of a word occur close to that word. may still occur across a span Examples: bigram: white wine, powerful tea >bigram: knock on the door; knock on X’s door

11 Non-compositionality Corpora and Statistical Methods white wine not really “white”, meaning not fully predictable from component words + syntax signal interpretation a term used in Intelligent Signal Processing: connotations go beyond compositional meaning Similarly: regression coefficient good practice guidelines Extreme cases: idioms such as kick the bucket meaning is completely frozen

12 Non-substitutability Corpora and Statistical Methods If a phrase is a collocation, we can’t substitute a word in the phrase for a near-synonym, and still have the same overall meaning. E.g.: white wine vs. yellow wine powerful tea vs. strong tea …

13 Non-modifiability Corpora and Statistical Methods Often, there are restrictions on inserting additional lexical items into the collocation, especially in the case of idioms. Example: kick the bucket vs. ?kick the large bucket NB: this is a matter of degree! non-idiomatic collocations are more flexible

14 Category restrictions Corpora and Statistical Methods Frequency alone doesn’t indicate collocational strength: by the is a very frequent phrase in English not a collocation Collocations tend to be formed from content words: A+N: powerful tea N+N: regression coefficient, mass demonstration N+PREP+N: degrees of freedom

15 Collocations “in a broad sense” Corpora and Statistical Methods In many statistical NLP applications, the term collocation is quite broadly understood: any phrase which is frequent/regular enough… proper names (New York) compound nouns (elevator operator) set phrases (part of speech) idioms (kick the bucket)

16 Why are collocations interesting? Corpora and Statistical Methods Several applications need to “know” about collocations: terminology extraction: technical or domain-specific phrases crop up frequently in text (oil prices) document classification: specialist phrases are good indicators of the topic of a text named entity recognition: names such as New York tend to occur together frequently; phrases like new toy don’t

17 Example application: Parsing Corpora and Statistical Methods She spotted the man with a pair of binoculars 1. [ VP spotted [ NP the man [ PP with a pair of binoculars]]] 2. [ VP spotted [ NP the man] [ PP with a pair of binoculars]] Parser might prefer (2) if spot/binoculars are frequent co- occurrences in a window of a certain width.

18 Example application: Generation Corpora and Statistical Methods NLG systems often need to map a semantic representation to a lexical/syntactic one. Shouldn’t use the wrong adjective-noun combinations: clean face vs. ?immaculate face Lapata et al. (1999): experiment asking people to rate different adjective-noun combinations frequency of the combination a strong predictor of people’s preferences argue that NLG systems need to be able to make contextually- informed decisions in lexical choice

19 Finding collocations in corpora: basic methods

20 Frequency-based approach Corpora and Statistical Methods Motivation: if two (or three, or…) words occur together a lot within some window, they’re a collocation Problems: frequent “collocations” under this definition include with the, onto a, etc. not very interesting…

21 Improving the frequency-based approach Corpora and Statistical Methods Justeson & Katz (1995): part of speech filter only look at word combinations of the “right” category: N + N: regression coefficient N + PRP + N: jack in (the) box … dramatically improves the results content-word combinations more likely to be phrases

22 Case study: strong vs. powerful Corpora and Statistical Methods See: Manning & Schutze `99, Sec 5.2 Motivation: try to distinguish the meanings of two quasi-synonyms data from New York Times corpus Basic strategy: find all bigrams where w1 = strong or powerful apply POS filter to remove strong on [crime], powerful in [industry] etc.

23 Case study (cont/d) Corpora and Statistical Methods Sample results from Manning & Schutze `99: f(strong support) = 50 f(strong supporter) = 10 f(powerful force) = 13 f(powerful computers) = 10 Teaser: would you also expect powerful supporter? what’s the difference between strong supporter and powerful supporter?

24 Limitations of frequency-based search Corpora and Statistical Methods Only work for fixed phrases But collocations can be “looser”, allowing interpolation of other words. knock on [the,X’s,a] door pull [a] punch Simple frequency won’t do for these: different interpolated words dilute the frequency.

25 Using mean and variance Corpora and Statistical Methods General idea: include bigrams even at a distance: w1Xw2 pull apunch Strategy: find co-occurrences of the two words in windows of varying length compute mean offset between w1 and w2 compute variance of offset between w1 and w2 if offsets are randomly distributed, then we have high variance and conclude that is not a collocation

26 Example outcomes (M&S `99) Corpora and Statistical Methods position of strong wrt opposition mean = -1.15, standard dev = 0.67 i.e. most occurrences are strong […] opposition position of strong wrt for mean = -1.12, standard dev = 2.15 i.e. for occurs anywhere around strong, SD is higher than mean. can get strong support for, for the strong support, etc.

27 More limitations of frequency Corpora and Statistical Methods If we use simple frequency or mean & variance, we have a good way of ranking likely collocations. But how do we know if a frequent pattern is frequent enough? Is it above what would be predicted by chance? We need to think in terms of hypothesis-testing. Given, we want to compare: The hypothesis that they are non-independent. The hypothesis that they are independent.

28 Preliminaries: Hypothesis testing and the binomial distribution

29 Permutations Suppose we have the 5 words {the, dog, ate, a, bone} How many permutations (possible orderings) are there of these words? the dog ate a bone dog the ate a bone … E.g. there are 5! = 120 ways of permuting 5 words.

30 Binomial coefficient Slight variation: How many different choices of three words are there out of these 5? This is known as an “n choose k” problem, in our case: “5 choose 3” For our problem, this gives us 10 ways of choosing three items out of 5

31 Bernoulli trials A Bernoulli (or binomial) trial is like a coin flip. Features: 1. There are two possible outcomes (not necessarily with the same likelihood), e.g. success/failure or 1/0. 2. If the situation is repeated, then the likelihoods of the two outcomes are stable.

32 Sampling with/out replacement Suppose we’re interested in the probability of pulling out a function word from a corpus of 100 words. we pull out words one by one without putting them back Is this a Bernoulli trial? we have a notion of success/failure: w is either a function word (“success”) or not (“failure”) but our chances aren’t the same across trials: they diminish since we sample without replacement

33 Cutting corners If the sample (e.g. the corpus) is large enough, then we can assume a Bernoulli situation even if we sample without replacement. Suppose our corpus has 52 million words Success = pulling out a function word Suppose there are 13 million function words First trial: p(success) =.25 Second trial: p(success) = 12,999,999/51,999,999 =.249 On very large samples, the chances remain relatively stable even without replacement.

34 Binomial probabilities - I Let π represent the probability of success on a Bernoulli trial (e.g. our simple word game on a large corpus). Then, p(failure) = 1 - π Problem: What are the chances of achieving success 3 times out of 5 trials? Assumption: each trial is independent of every other. (Is this assumption reasonable?)

35 Binomial probabilities - II How many ways are there of getting success three times out of 5? Several: SSSFF, SFSFS, SFSSF, … To estimate the number of possible ways of getting k outcomes from n possibilities, we use the binomial coefficient:

36 Binomial probabilities - III “5 choose 3” gives 10. Given independence, each of these sequences is equally likely. What’s the probability of a sequence? it’s an AND problem (multiplication rule) P(SSSFF) = πππ (1- π )(1 – π ) = π 3 (1- π ) 2 P(SFSFS) = π (1- π ) π (1- π ) π = π 3 (1- π ) 2 (they all come out the same)

37 Binomial probabilities - IV The binomial distribution states that: given n Bernoulli trials, with probability π of success on each trial, the probability of getting exactly k successes is: probability of each success Number of different ways of getting k successes probability of k successes out of n

38 Expected value and variance Expected value: where π is our probability of success Expected value of X over n trials Variance of X over n trials

39 Using the t-test for collocation discovery

40 The logic of hypothesis testing The typical scenario in hypothesis testing compares two hypotheses: 1. The research hypothesis 2. A null hypothesis The idea is to set up our experiment (study, etc) in such a way that: If we show the null hypothesis to be false then we can affirm our research hypothesis with a certain degree of confidence

41 H0 for collocation studies There is no real association between w1 and w2, i.e. occurrence of is no more likely than chance. More formally: H0: P(w1 & w2) = P(w1)P(w2) i.e. P(w1) and P(w2) are independent

42 Some more on hypothesis testing Our research hypothesis (H1): are strong collocates P(w1 & w2) > P(w1)P(w2) A null hypothesis H0 P(w1 & w2) = P(w1)P(w2) How do we know whether our results are sufficient to affirm H1? I.e. how big is our risk of wrongly falsifying H0?

43 The notion of significance We generally fix a “level of confidence” in advance. In many disciplines, we’re happy with being 95% confident that the result we obtain is correct. So we have a 5% chance of error. Therefore, we state our results at p = 0.05 “The probability of wrongly rejecting H0 is 5% (0.05)”

44 Tests for significance Many of the tests we use involve: 1. having a prior notion of what the mean/variance of a population is, according to H0 2. computing the mean/variance on our sample of the population 3. checking whether the sample mean/variance is different from the sample predicted by H0, at 95% confidence.

45 The t-test: strategy obtain mean (x) and variance (s 2 ) for a sample H0: sample is drawn from a population with mean μ and variance σ 2 estimate the t value: this compares the sample mean/variance to the expected (population) mean/variance under H0 check if any difference found is significant enough to reject H0

46 Computing t calculate difference between sample mean and expected population mean scale the difference by the variance Assumption: population is normally distributed. If t is big enough, we reject H0. The magnitude of t given our sample size N is simply looked up in a table. Tables tell us what the level of significance is (p-value, or likelihood of making a Type 1 error, wrongly rejecting H0).

47 Example: new companies We think of our corpus as a series of bigrams, and each sample we take is an indicator variable (Bernoulli trial): value = 1 if a bigram is new companies value = 0 otherwise Compute P(new) and P(companies) using standard MLE. H0: P(new companies) = P(new)P(companies)

48 Example continued We have computed the likelihood of our bigram of interest under H0. Since this is a Bernoulli Trial, this is also our expected mean. We then compute the actual sample probability of (new companies). Compute t and check significance

49 Uses of the t-test Often used to rank candidate collocations, rather than compute significance. Stop word lists must be used, else all bigrams will be significant. e.g. M&S report 824 out of 831 bigrams that pass the significance test. Reason: language is just not random regularities mean that if the corpus is large enough, all bigrams will occur together regularly and often enough to be significant. Kilgarriff (2005): Any null hypothesis will be rejected on a large enough corpus.

50 Extending the t-test to compare samples Variation on the original problem: what co-occurrence relations are best to distinguish between two words, w1 and w1’ that are near-synonyms? e.g. strong vs. powerful Strategy: find all bigrams and e.g. strong tea, strong support check, for each w1, if it occurs significantly more often with w2, versus w2’. NB. This is a two-sample t-test

51 Two-sample t-test: details H0: For any w1, the probabilities of and is the same. i.e. μ (expected difference) = 0 Strategy: extract sample of and assume they are independent compute mean and SD for each sample compute t check for significance: is the magnitude of the difference large enough? Formula:

52 Simplifying under binomial assumptions On large samples, variance in the binomial distribution approaches the mean. I.e.: (similarly for the other sample mean) Therefore:

53 Concrete example: strong vs. powerful (M&S, p. 167); NY Times Words occurring significantly more often with powerful than strong Words occurring significantly more often with strong than powerful

54 Criticisms of the t-test Assumes that the probabilities are normally distributed. This is probably not the case in linguistic data, where probabilities tend to be very large or very small. Alternative: chi-squared test ( Χ 2 ) compare differences between expected and observed frequencies (e.g. of bigrams)

55 The chi-square test

56 Example Imagine we’re interested in whether poor performance is a good collocation. H0: frequency of poor performance is no different from the expected frequency if each word occurs independently. Find frequencies of bigrams containing poor, performance and poor performance. compare actual to expected frequencies check if the value is high enough to reject H0

57 Example continued f(w1= poor)F(w1 =/= poor) f(w2=performance)15 (poor performance) 1,230 (bad performance) F(w2 =/= performance)3,580 (poor people) 12,000 (all other bigrams) OBSERVED FREQUENCIES Expected frequencies need to be computed for each cell: E.g. expected value for cell (1,1) poor performance:

58 Computing the value The chi-squared value is the sum of differences of observed and expected frequencies, scaled by expected frequencies. Value is once again looked up in a table to check if degree of confidence (p-value) is acceptable. If so, we conclude that the dependency between w1 and w2 is significant.

59 More applications of this statistic Kilgarriff and Rose 1998 use chi-square as a measure of corpus similarity draw up an n (row)*2 (column) table columns correspond to corpora rows correspond to individual types compare the difference in counts between corpora H0: corpora are drawn from the same underlying linguistic population (e.g. register or variety) corpora will be highly similar if the ratio of counts for each word is roughly constant. This uses lexical variation to compute corpus-similarity.

60 Limitations of t-test and chi-square Not easily interpretable a large chi-square or t value suggests a large difference but makes more sense as a comparative measure, rather than in absolute terms t-test is problematic because of the normality assumption chi-square doesn’t work very well for small frequencies (by convention, we don’t calculate it if the expected value for any of the cells is less than 5) but n-grams will often be infrequent!

61 Likelihood ratios for collocation discovery

62 Rationale A likelihood ratio is the ratio of two probabilities indicates how much more likely one hypothesis is compared to another Notation: c 1 = C(w1) c 2 = C(w2) c 12 = C( ) Hypotheses: H0: P(w2|w1) = p = P(w2|¬w1) H1: P(w2|w1) = p1 P(w2|¬w1) = p2 p1 =/= p2

63 H0H0H1H1 P(w2|w1) P(w2|¬w1) Prob. that c 12 bigrams out of c 1 are Prob. that c 2 - c 12 out of N- c 1 bigrams are ) Computing the likelihood ratio

64 The likelihood (odds) that a hypothesis H is correct is L(H).

65 Computing the Likelihood ratio We usually compute the log of the ratio: Usually expressed as: because, for v. large samples, this is roughly equivalent to a Χ 2 value

66 Interpreting the ratio Suppose that the likelihood ratio for some bigram is x. This says: If we make the hypothesis that w2 is somehow dependent on w1, then we expect it to occur x times more than its actual base rate of occurrence would predict. This ratio is also better for sparse data. we can use the estimate as an approximate chi-square value even when expected frequencies are small.

67 Concrete example: bigrams involving powerful (M&S, p. 174) Source: NY Times corpus (N=14.3m) Note: sparse data can still have a high log likelihood value! Interpreting -2 log l as chi-squared allows us to reject H0, even for small samples (e.g. powerful cudgels)

68 Relative frequency ratios An extension of the same logic of a likelihood ratio used to compare collocations across corpora Let be our bigram of interest. Let C1 and C2 be two corpora: p1 = P( ) in C1 p2 = P( ) in C2. r= p1/p2 gives an indication of the relative likelihood of in C1 and C2.

69 Example application Manning and Schutze (p.176) compare: C1: NY Times texts from 1990 C2: NY Times texts from 1989 Bigram occurs 44 times in C2, but only 2 times in C1, so r = 0.03 The big difference is due to 1989 papers dealing more with the fall of the Berlin Wall.

70 Summary We’ve now considered two forms of hypothesis testing: t-test chi-square Also, log-likelihood ratios as measures of relative probability under different hypotheses. Next, we begin to look at the problem of lexical acquisition.

71 References M. Lapata, S. McDonald & F. Keller (1999). Determinants of Adjective- Noun plausibility. Proceedings of the 9th Conference of the European Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics, EACL-99 A. Kilgarriff (2005). Language is never, ever, ever random. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory 1(2): 263 Church, K. and Hanks, P. (1990). Word association norms, mutual information and lexicography. Computational Linguistics 16(1).


Download ppt "Albert Gatt Corpora and Statistical Methods. In this lecture Corpora and Statistical Methods We have considered distributions of words and lexical variation."

Similar presentations


Ads by Google