Download presentation
Presentation is loading. Please wait.
Published byLeon Bryan Modified over 9 years ago
1
1. Introduction 2.Course Information 3.Study Design 4.Looking at Data Today’s Topics Introduction to the Practice of Statistics Ch. 1, 2.5, 3.2 MBP1010 – Jan. 4, 2011
2
(1) How can we describe and draw meaning from a collection of data? (2) How can we infer information about the whole population when we know data from only some of the population (a sample)? Meaning from Data
3
- science of understanding data and making decisions in the face of variability and uncertainty - statistics is NOT a field of mathematics
4
Statistical Thinking -humans are good at recognizing patterns and there is real danger of over-interpreting patterns that are merely due to the play of chance (false leads) - role of statistics - to reject chance as an explanation so that we can have reasonable assurance that patterns seen are worthy of interpretation
5
Statistical Thinking - explore data prior to analysis - think about context and design - reasoning behind standard statistical methods Interpretation/Conclusions
6
1. Study designs/Looking at data 2. Concepts of statistical inference and hypothesis testing 3. Specific statistical tests - 1 and 2 sample test for continuous and categorical data - correlation, regression and ANOVA 4. Other Topics - eg sensitivity/specifiicity, survival analysis, logistic regression 5. Bioinformatics Course Overview
7
Changes to MBP1010 this year Good news: doing less actual statistical analysis focus more on concepts/interpretation Bad news: short time frame to implement changes Department has made attendance at lectures mandatory. Good news/Bad news!
8
What statistical software is available in your lab? What software does you supervisor recommend? What statistical software have you used? Email to: lmartin@uhnres.utoronto.ca by Mon Jan 10 at the latest Information Requested
9
Course Information Tutorials: Thursdays 2 to 3:30 pm OCI 7-605 First tutorial: Jan 13, 2011 TA: Dave Stock Lectures: Tuesdays 1 to 3 pm 620 University, 7-709 Course Website – U of T Blackboard UTORiD and password; U of T email address
10
Updated course information and schedule posted at website no lecture or tutorial (Jan 25/27) Updated marking scheme 3 Biostatistics Assignments: 5+15+15=35% Biostatistics Exam:30% Bioinformatics Assignment30% Participation 5%
11
Resources see website for electronic resources Introduction to the Practice of Statistics (5th Edition), by Moore, DS and McCabe, GP). Presenting medical statistics from proposal to publication: A step-by-step guide. by Janet Peacock and Sally KerryJanet PeacockSally Kerry
13
Can what we eat influence our risk of cancer? The case of dietary fat and breast cancer Study Design Posted on website: New York Times article Searching for clarity: A primer on medical studies
15
What should we do next?
16
An observational study observes individuals and measures variables of interest but does not attempt to influence the responses. Observational Studies
17
Case/control and cohort studies common in cancer research (epidemiology) - outcome is binary: cancer/ no cancer Observational studies often examine factors associated with continuous outcome variables - eg association of body weight or diet with hormone levels - calcium intake and blood pressure
18
X X X X X X X 0 X X X 0 0 00 0 0 0 0 0 Exposure eg diet Case Control Study Exposure eg diet
19
00 0 00 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 00 0 X0 0 0 X 0 0 X 0 0 X Cohort Study Exposure eg diet Cancer (yes/no)
20
Relative Risk Compare risk of disease in those with highest versus lowest intake RR = 1.0 no association RR = 1.4 1.4 times the risk 40% higher risk RR = 0.8 20% lower risk
21
a. Total Fat Odds Ratio or Relative Risk Case Control: Challier (1998) DeStefani (1998) Ewertz (1990) Franceschi (1996) Graham (1982) Graham (1991) Hirohata (1985) Hirohata (1987) (Caucasian) Hirohata (1987) (Japanese) Ingram (1991) Katsouyanni (1988) Katsouyanni (1994) Landa (1994) Lee (1991) Levi (1993) Mannisto (1999) Martin-Moreno (1994) Miller (1978) Núñez (1996) Potischman (1998) Pryor (1989) Richardson (1991) Rohan (1988) Shun-Zhang (1990) Toniolo (1989) Trichopoulou (1995) van't Veer (1990,1991) Wakai (2000) Witte (1997) Yuan (1995) Zaridze (1991) Case Control Summary Cohort : Gaard (1995) Graham (1992) Holmes (1999) Howe (1991) Jones (1987) Knekt (1990) Kushi (1992) Thiébaut (2001 ) Toniolo (1994) van den Brandt (1993) Velie (2000) Wolk (1998) Cohort Summary All Studies Summary Bingham (2003) Cho (2003) 0123456131415
22
Interpretation Suppose we find that women who eat a low fat diet tend to have lower risk of breast cancer. Can we conclude that the fat in the diet is responsible for the lower risk of breast cancer?
23
Interpretation Suppose we find that women who eat a low fat diet tend to have lower risk of breast cancer. Can we conclude that the fat in the diet is responsible for the lower risk of breast cancer? No. Other factors may be responsible for the association with dietary fat (confounding)
24
Problem of Confounding Suppose A is associated with B: This may be because: A causes B B causes A X is associated with both A and B X need not be a cause of either A or B
25
Problem of Confounding -women who eat more dietary fat may differ from those who less fat (eg. weight, exercise, other dietary factors) -these factors may influence the risk of breast cancer In our dietary fat example:
26
Trying to control for confounding - measure potential confounders eg. measure weight and physical activity -“control” for possible confounders in analysis - but…what about confounding with variables we don’t know exist or can’t measure?
27
An observational study observes individuals and measures variables of interest but does not attempt to influence the responses. Association between variables a response variable, even if it is very strong, is not good evidence of a cause and effect link between variables Observational Studies Correlation is not causation
28
Basic principles of experimental design 1.Formulate question/goal in advance 2. Comparison/control 3. Replication 4. Randomization 5. Stratification (or blocking)
29
Example Question: Does salted drinking water affect blood pressure (BP) in mice? Experiment: 1Provide treatment - water containing 1% NaCl for 14 days 1.Measure outcome - BP 29
30
Comparison/control Good experiments are comparative. Compare BP in mice fed salt water to BP in mice fed plain water. Ideally, the experimental group is compared to concurrent controls (rather than to historical controls). 30
31
Why replicate? Reduce the effect of uncontrolled variation (i.e., increase precision). Quantify uncertainty. A related point: An estimate of effect is of no value without some statement of the uncertainty in the estimate. 31
32
Randomization Experimental subjects (“units”) should be assigned to treatment groups at random. At random does not mean haphazardly. One needs to explicitly randomize using A computer, or Coins, dice or cards. 32
33
Why randomize? Avoid bias. –For example: the first six mice you grab may have intrinsically higher BP. Control the role of chance. –Randomization allows the later use of probability theory, and so gives a solid foundation for statistical analysis. 33
34
Stratification Suppose that measurements will be made in males and females AND You anticipate a difference in response between males and females – Randomize within males and females separately - any systematic difference by sex removed - this is sometimes called “blocking”. -Take account of the difference between males and females in analysis: - helps control variability
35
Randomization and stratification If you can (and want to), fix a variable. – e.g., study only men or women or a single strain of animal If you don’t fix a variable, stratify on it. – e.g., randomize treatment men and women If you can neither fix nor stratify a variable, randomize to treatment.
36
Other points Blinding –Measurements made by people can be influenced by unconscious biases. –Ideally, measurements should be made without knowledge of the treatment applied. Internal controls –use the subjects themselves as their own controls (e.g., consider the response after vs. before treatment). –Why? Increased precision. 36
37
Other points Representativeness –Are the subjects/tissues you are studying really representative of the population you want to study? –Ideally, your study material is a random sample from the population of interest. 37
38
Summary comparative - control group Unbiased –Randomization –Blinding High precision –Replication –Blocking Simple –Protect against mistakes Able to estimate uncertainty –Replication –Randomization Characteristics of good experiments: 38
39
Jackson et al. Nutr.Cancer, 1998 Dietary fat and mammary tumors in Sprague-Dawley rats (n=30 per diet group) Randomized Design
40
Dietary fat and fiber and mammary tumors in Sprague-Dawley rats (n=30) Factorial Experiment
41
Women’s Health Initiative (US) - 48,835 postmenopausal women - followed for 8-12 years Diet and Breast Cancer Prevention Study - 4793 high risk women - followed for 7-17 years Randomized Clinical Trials in Humans - Dietary Fat and Breast Cancer
42
Women’s Health Initiative - Postmenopausal women (50-79 years of age) - n=48,835; follow-up 8-12 years - randomized 40:60 intervention and control - group dietary counselling - follow up for breast cancer
43
Copyright restrictions may apply. Prentice, R. L. et al. JAMA 2006;295:629-642. Kaplan-Meier Estimates of the Cumulative Hazard for Invasive Breast Cancer
44
Eligible Subjects Identified (> 50% density) Prerandomization Assessment Intervention Control (n=2,343) (n=2,350) Annual Visits demo/anthro data diet records non fasting serum Follow up until Dec 2005 (7-17 years per subject) breast cancer incidence
45
Cumulative breast cancer hazards and odds ratios according to randomized group. Martin L J et al. Cancer Res 2011;71:123-133 ©2011 by American Association for Cancer Research
46
Practical Issues: - long (particularly for cancer outcomes!) - expensive - limited in “treatment” options Randomized Clinical Trials in Humans
47
- highly selected subjects - selection criteria and motivation - subject/investigator blinding - subjects drop out -compliance? - other changes with intervention? Randomized Clinical Trials in Humans Other issues:
48
salt intake? food intake? weight? activity? Does salted drinking water affect BP in mice?
49
Main Points - primary interest is causal relationships between variables - observational studies show associations only - randomized studies best for causation but are not without challenges - totality of evidence important
51
What’s in the dataset? What are the observations (individuals)? Eg people, animals, cells, countries How many observations are in the dataset? How many observations should there be? Are the observations independent? - repeated in an individudal?
52
What are the variables? What is their exact definition? How were they measured? What are the units of measurement? What type of variables? What’s in the dataset?
53
Main Types of Variables Categorical: - include nominal and dichotomous variables - qualitative difference between values - eg sex (male/female), smoker/non smoker Continuous: - quantitative - equal distance between each value - eg blood pressure, age, dietary fat Ordinal variables can be ordered but they do not have specific numeric values, eg scales, ratings
54
Continuous Variables Examining a distribution: overall pattern can be described by shape, centre and spread in a graph of data look for overall pattern and striking deviations from the pattern outlier – individual value that falls outside the overall pattern
57
Stem and Leaf Plots - displays distribution of small/moderate amounts of data - includes the actual numerical values Example data: Blood pressure data in 21 patients 107 110 123 129 112 111 107 112 136 102 123 109 112 102 98 114 119 112 110 117 130 9 : 8 10 : 22779 11 : 0012222479 12 : 339 13 : 06 Stem (all but last digit) Leaf (last digit)
58
(left)—Serum albumin values in 248 adults FIG 2 (right)—Normal distribution with the same mean and standard deviation as the serum albumin values. Altman D G, Bland J M BMJ 1995;310:298 ©1995 by British Medical Journal Publishing Group
59
Importance of Normal Distribution* 1. Distributions of real data are often close to normal. 2. Mathematically easy to work with so many statistical tests are designed for normal (or close to normal) distributions). 3. If the mean and SD of a normal distribution are known, you can make quantitative predictions about the population. * also called Gaussian curve
60
Describing Distributions with Numbers
62
Blood Pressure Data: n= 21 measurements mean = 2395/21 = 114 median = observation 11 = 112 98 102 102 107 107 109 110 110 111 112 112 112 112 114 117 119 123 123 129 130 136
63
2 8 15 3 29 5 8 1 20 17 6 5 31 44 10 12 23 62 Mean versus Median - skewed data 0: 12355688 1: 0257 2: 039 3: 1 4: 4 5: 6: 2 Stem Plot Mean = 16.7 Median = 11
64
100 102 104 105 106 112 114 115 116 125 100 104 109 115 125 BP data; n = 10 Min Q1 Median Q3 Max
66
75% quantile 25% quantile Median IQR 1.5xIQR Everything above or below are considered outliers
67
Dot Plot
68
Measures of Spread - range of data set: largest - smallest value - interquartile range (IQR): 3rd minus 1st quartile - sample variance and standard deviation
69
Deviation from the Mean
72
Choosing a summary Five-number summary -skewed distribution - outliers x and s (mean and std dev.) - reasonably symmetric - free of outliers
74
Extreme Observations or Outliers - rule of thumb 1.5 x IQR for potential outliers - observations that stand apart from the overall pattern (not just extreme values) - do not automatically delete outliers - try to explain them - an error in measurement or in recording data - an usual occurrence - describe outliers, what you do with them and what their effect is
75
1.5 x 3.5(IQR) = 5.25 75th (11.46) + 5.25 = 16.71 19.9 MJ Energy expenditure in 29 women measured by doubly labelled water (MJ per day).
76
What did we do about the outlier? - checked recording/calculations/data entry - unusual occurrence? - biologically plausible? - re-measured laboratory samples - analysis with and without outlier - described all above in paper
77
Data Display
78
Data presentation Bad plot Good plot 78
79
% Dietary Fat Intervention Control Dietary fat intake in the intervention and control groups (n=150 intervention and 187 control)
81
How to Display Data Badly H Wainer (1984) How to display data badly. American Statistician 38(2):137-147 - posted at website -use of Microsoft Excel and Powerpoint has resulted in remarkable advances in the field (of poor data display)
82
The aim of good data graphics: Display data accurately and clearly. Some rules for displaying data badly: – Display as little information as possible. – Obscure what you do show (with chart junk). – Use pseudo-3d and color gratuitously. – Make a pie chart (preferably in color and 3d). – Use a poorly chosen scale. General principles
85
Pay attention to scale! Same data, different scale
86
Displaying data well Be accurate and clear. Let the data speak. – Show as much information as possible, taking care not to obscure the message. Science not sales. – Avoid unnecessary frills — esp. gratuitous 3d. In tables, every digit should be meaningful.
87
Further reading – Data Display ER Tufte http://www.edwardtufte.com/tufte/ (1983) The visual display of quantitative Information. (1990) Envisioning information. (1997) Visual explanations. WS Cleveland (1993) Visualizing data. Hobart Press. WS Cleveland (1994) The elements of graphing data. CRC Press.
88
What statistical software is available in your lab? What software does you supervisor recommend? What statistical software have you used? Email to: lmartin@uhnres.utoronto.ca by Mon Jan 10 at the latest Information Requested
Similar presentations
© 2025 SlidePlayer.com. Inc.
All rights reserved.