Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Presentation is loading. Please wait.

Incentive and the Stop-Signal Task and Associations with Temperament Rebecca J. Mazzullo, Craig R. Colder 1, Larry W. Hawk 1, Jr., and Liliana J. Lengua.

Similar presentations


Presentation on theme: "Incentive and the Stop-Signal Task and Associations with Temperament Rebecca J. Mazzullo, Craig R. Colder 1, Larry W. Hawk 1, Jr., and Liliana J. Lengua."— Presentation transcript:

1 Incentive and the Stop-Signal Task and Associations with Temperament Rebecca J. Mazzullo, Craig R. Colder 1, Larry W. Hawk 1, Jr., and Liliana J. Lengua 2 Rebecca J. Mazzullo 1, Craig R. Colder 1, Larry W. Hawk 1, Jr., and Liliana J. Lengua 2 1 University at Buffalo, The State University of New York 2 University of Washington INTRODUCTION This research was supported by a grant from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01 DA019631) awarded to Dr. Craig Colder.. The content of this poster is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute on Drug Abuse. Correspondence should be addressed to Rebecca Mazzullo, B.A. Department of Psychology, SUNY at Buffalo, Buffalo, New York 14260 Email: rjm33@buffalo.edu Our findings suggest some convergence between SST performance and parent reports of temperament, and some unexpected findings. We found a general pattern suggesting that all three components of effortful control were associated with high levels of SST assessed inhibitory control. Children characterized by good effortful control are viewed by parents as having the capacity to focus attention, restrain impulsive responses, and shift attention when needed. Our data suggest that these are all important aspects of successful inhibition during the SST. In support of one of our main hypotheses, children characterized by high levels of effortful control demonstrated high levels of inhibitory control on the SST regardless of whether incentives were present or not. This suggests that rewards are not necessary for children characterized by high effortful control to perform well on the task. Contrary to our hypothesis, we found that children characterized by high levels of parent reported SP improved their inhibitory control when reward was available. It is possible that our incentive condition generated strong expectations of reward, and that poor performance indicated by a low point total was experienced as frustrative non-reward. This may have motivated high SP children to improve inhibition. This would be consistent with Gray (1987), who suggested that frustrative non-reward can activate the behavioral inhibition system (BIS). Our findings did not support an association between high levels of parent reported SR and improved inhibitory control during the reward condition. SR is thought to measure behavior reflecting activation of a behavioral approach system (BAS), which is engaged by signals of reward. Accordingly, we expected children characterized by high levels of SR to be particularly responsive to the incentive manipulation. However, the behavioral response measured in the SST is inhibition, which is BIS, not BAS mediated. Thus, despite rewards being present, our findings suggest that the SST is more likely to engage the BIS than the BAS. There may be some clinical implications of our findings. Effortful control (and it’s components) provided good convergence with performance on the SST, and like SST assessed inhibitory control, it is an important individual difference to consider when planning interventions aimed at children who are at-risk for externalizing problems. Children who are low in effortful control tend to use ineffective coping responses and have higher risk for negative individual and interpersonal outcomes, are more aggressive, and experience more peer problems (Loukas, 2004). The SST may provide a useful laboratory analogue method for teaching children good effortful control. Therefore, it would be beneficial for additional longitudinal research to examine whether change in effortful control is associated with change in SST performance and the role that incentives may play in the development of improved self-regulation. Participants & Procedures. The sample included 118 11- to 12- year-old children taken from a larger longitudinal study recruited from the community in Erie County, New York using random-digit-dialing procedures. Adolescents with any disabilities precluding them from understanding or completing the interviews were excluded from the study. The majority of the sample was female (62.7%) and Caucasian (74.6%). The entire session lasted for two-and-a-half hours and families were compensated $75 upon completion of the session. Measures. Parents reported on their child’s temperament using the Revised Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire for Children (SPSRQ-CR) and the Revised Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire (EATQ-R). The SPSRQ-CR is composed of two scales, including sensitivity to punishment (SP), which measures behavioral inhibition and reaction to the threat of punishment or failure, and sensitivity to reward (SR), which primarily measures impulsivity, sensation- seeking, and reward responsivity. The EATQ-R was included in this study to assess temperament related to self-regulation. For our purposes, we used the activation control (the capacity to perform an action), attention (the ability to sustain attention on a task), and inhibitory control (competence at suppressing actions or responses) subscales from the EATQ-R. Additionally, we considered the broader, second-order factor of effortful control, which combines activation control, attention, and inhibitory control into one dimension. Children completed the SST, which was adapted to include a reward incentive condition (Logan, 1997). The two conditions were administered in a fixed order: 1). children received no feedback/no incentive (NI), and 2). incentives/feedback (INC) were provided and response speed was rewarded with more points. Points accumulated during the task were later redeemed for a prize. Data reduction and analysis. The variable of interest used to measure inhibitory control from the SST was Stop Signal Reaction Time (SSRT). The SSRT is an estimate of the latency of inhibiting a prepotent response that is calculated by subtracting the mean stop delay (the delay between the stop signal and the go signal) from the mean reaction time (the average time it took to respond to the go signal) and was calculated in the No Incentive (NI) and Incentive (INC) conditions. Difficulty in inhibiting a response when a stop signal is presented (I.e. poor inhibitory control) results in a long SSRT, where a shorter SSRT indicates good inhibitory control. We also calculated a change score (no incentive minus incentive) to represent changes in inhibitory control in response to incentives. For the temperament measures, the items within each scale were averaged to form scaled scores. Table 1: Correlations Between Parent Reported Temperament and SST Inhibitory Control METHODS RESULTS DISCUSSION ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS & CONTACT INFORMATION Note. SSRT= Stop Signal Reaction Time, NI= No Incentive Condition, INC=Incentive Condition. * p <.05, ** p <.01. REFERENCES Colder, C., O’Connor, R. (2004). Gray's Reinforcement Sensitivity Model and Child Psychopathology: Laboratory and Questionnaire Assessment of the BAS and BIS. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32 (4), 435- 451. Gray, J. (1987). Control and Impulsivity: A Commentary. Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 493-509. Logan, G., Schachar, R., Tannock, R. (1997). Impulsivity and Inhibitory Control. Psychological Sciences, 8 (1), 60-64. Loukas, A. and Robinson, S. (2004). Examining the Moderating Role of Perceived School Climate in Early Adolescent Adjustment. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 14 (2), 209-233. Rothbart, M.K. et al. (2004). Temperament and self regulation. Handbook of Self Regulation. (Baumeister, R.F. and Vohs, K.D., eds), 357–370, Guilford Press. SSRT (NI) SSRT (INC) SSRT Δ ACTIVATION CONTROL-.22*-.21* -.00 INHIBITORY CONTROL-.25**-.12 ATTENTION -.22* -.24**.03 EFFORTFUL CONTROL -.27**-.24*-.03 SENSITIVITY PUNISHMENT -.03-.16.19* SENSITIVITY REWARD.12.13.05 Abstract. The Stop-Signal Task (SST) task has been used to assess inhibitory control, and recently the task has been adapted to examine the impact of incentives on inhibition. Given the importance of inhibitory control and sensitivity to incentives in models of disinhibitory psychopathology (e.g., ADHD and Conduct Disorder), it is important to have multiple methods of assessing these individual differences. This study examined the association of parent reports of child temperament with inhibitory control and the effects of motivational incentives on inhibitory control assessed using the SST. Results suggested that parent reports of effortful control were associated with SST inhibitory control. Also, SST inhibitory control improved with the introduction of reward, and this was particularly true for children characterized by high levels of parent reported sensitivity to punishment. Findings supported convergence of measures of inhibitory control, and some unexpected associations with respect to sensitivity to incentives. Background. Poor inhibitory control and hypersensitivity to reward are central to many neurocognitive accounts of externalizing behavior problems (e.g., ADHD and Conduct Disorder). The stop-signal paradigm assesses inhibitory control by measuring a person’s ability to inhibit an ongoing action. Recently, it has been adapted to examine the effects of incentives, making it a useful tool for examining core processes underlying childhood externalizing behavior problems. Nonetheless, it is important to establish multiple methods for assessing these theoretically important individual differences. Several temperament and personality theories include dimensions of inhibitory control and sensitivity to incentives (Gray, 1987 and Rothbart, 2004), and parent report questionnaires have been developed to assess these individual differences in children. The goal of this study was to examine how parent report assessments of temperament relate to child performance on the SST. Hypotheses. It was hypothesized that improvements in SST inhibitory control with the introduction of reward would be associated with high levels of parent reported sensitivity to reward. We also predicted that good SST inhibitory control would be associated with high levels of parent reported effortful control and sensitivity to punishment, regardless of incentives. Short SSRT (good inhibitory control) during both the NI and INC conditions was associated with high levels of parent reported activation control, attention, and effortful control. Short SSRT (good inhibitory control) in the NI condition was associated with high levels of parent reported inhibitory control. Decreases in SSRT (improved inhibitory control) from the NI to INC condition were associated with high levels of parent reported SP. SST variables were not associated with parent reported SR.


Download ppt "Incentive and the Stop-Signal Task and Associations with Temperament Rebecca J. Mazzullo, Craig R. Colder 1, Larry W. Hawk 1, Jr., and Liliana J. Lengua."

Similar presentations


Ads by Google